Syntax of gjâ-zym-byn
Overview of gjâ-zym-byn grammar
gjâ-zym-byn is a fiendishly difficult language that I cannot wrap my
mind around. — Jörg
Rhiemeier
This document describes the syntax of gjâ-zym-byn, and its
inflectional morphology, what there is of it (a handful of verb
endings). Another document describes
its derivational morphology.
Typologically, gjâ-zym-byn (gzb) primarily follows object-verb-subject
(OVS) word order, and has a mix of head-final and head-initial
aspects: adjectives/adverbs follow the words they modify, and main
verbs follow auxiliary verbs, but postpositional phrases precede the
words they modify. It is an agglutinative language, with the vast
majority of morphemes consisting of one syllable (though some
noun roots are two or even three syllables, and some bound morphemes
in postpositions and conjunctions are only one phoneme); it has an index
of synthesis of 1.729
and an index of agglutinativity of 1.0. gzb
could be tentatively described as a fluid-S active
language (as opposed to accusative
or ergative),
though this label doesn't fit perfectly: it marks agents, patients,
experiencers, and so forth for fairly specific semantic roles rather
than abstracting many semantic roles into generic subject and object
syntactic roles.
As
for conlang
typology, gzb could be described as a whimsical,
idiosyncratic engelang, or a
highly schematic, non-naturalistic artlang. On Ray
Brown's Gnoli
Maxwell Triangle, gzb would probably be a greenish cyan, on or
near the upper left edge of the triangle, a little nearer the artlang
vertice than the engelang vertice. According to the Wikipedia
typology of artlangs, gzb is a personal
language; Rick Harrison's term "heartlang" might apply
as well. The terms "hermetic language" or "langue
close", used by Javant Biarujia, Robert Dessaix and Paul
Burgess, seem less apt for gzb; at this point in its history, anyway,
most of the corpus of gzb (consisting of entries in my journal) is
private and secret, but the language itself isn't, and the public part
of the corpus (original and translated literature) is getting fairly
significant. Some people consider gzb to be
a conlex,
but I don't use that word for it myself, mainly because I don't know
the conlexicist subculture well enough to understand how they use the
word, and because some of the criteria the term's proponents list don't quite
fit gzb or my use of it.
gzb's lexicon is primarily a
priori, with a moderate fraction a posteriori (mostly
names of animals and plants, taken from scientific Latin); the grammar
is entirely a priori, not based on any specific language or
language family (though there are scads of unintentional similarities
to various natlangs).
Basic root words are by themselves nouns. You can add suffixes to
make verbs, modifiers (adjectives/adverbs), conjunctions and
postpositions from them. Grammatical particles include a core set of
spacetime postpositions; several kinds of conjunctions; general
modifiers (adverbs or adjectives, according to context); pronouns; and
suffixes. A nominalizer clitic can turn the modifier particles and
postpositions into nouns.
Case, number, gender, tense, and mood aren't shown by grammatical
inflection or affix, but by postpositions and modifier particles.
Most aspect distinctions are marked with adverbial particles or
postpositions, but some with suffixes.
Syntactic roles (case) are shown by postpositions and word order.
Common sentence types include topic-comment, topic-state,
patient-verb-agent, and topic-verb-experiencer. There are no abstract
subject/object markers, consequently no passive voice. However, I
still find it useful to use the terms "subject" and
"object" in describing gzb grammar, each being an umbrella
term for several case roles with common morphosyntactic properties
(though the sets overlap a bit); pronouns in the subject cases can be
incorporated into the verb, and nouns in the subject cases can,
if they come at the end of a clause, omit the case postposition.
Nouns and pronouns in the object cases don't exhibit those
behaviors.
The phrases of a sentence (verb, agent, patient, object-of-attention,
experiencer, topic, state, comment, temporal and locative complements,
etc.) can generally come in any order, but object-verb-subject is the
default unmarked word-order, with temporal and local complements most
commonly preceding the object.
gzb is a verb-drop language; in sentences where the action, process,
state etc. is clear enough from the postpositional phrases used,
the verb may be omitted (and in some types of sentences there is
in fact no room for a verb).
Types of root morphemes
gjâ-zym-byn has its own suitable terms for the functionally
distinct kinds of root morpheme; some of these correspond to
"parts of speech" in traditional grammar.
{gun} are content root-words; names of kinds of people, animals, things,
states, qualities, actions, processes, numbers, ideas, and so forth.
{gun} contain the vowels |î|, |ě|, |â|, |u|, |y|, or |ĭ| or their
nasal forms. Standing alone, or compounded with each other, {gun} fit into the
traditional category of nouns. In theory, this is the language's only
open class morpheme type
(but in practice, I'm still adding to the other classes from time to time
as well, though at a much slower rate; I even added more pronouns as late
as April 2005).
{jum} are modifier particles; they're used like adjectives &
adverbs (or articles) to change the meaning of a preceding word, or
specify which of several possible referents is meant. They contain
one of the vowels |ǒ| or |e|, and have allomorphic forms with the
nasal vowels |ǒň| or |eň| which occur after root words
with a nasal vowel (vowel harmony). (Because they obey vowel harmony
with respect to the preceding word, like suffixes, I call these
modifier particles clitics. Feel free to
yell at me if I'm using that term incorrectly.)
{ŋwĭm} are pronouns. Most are clicks or ejectives ({Ќ, ť}...);
a few look like {jum}, a consonant followed by |e|.
{ðujm} are conjunctions. They can have one of the oral
vowels |ǒ| or |e|, or the nasals |iň| or |oň|. Generally you can tell the
nature of a {ðujm} - whether it shows truth-values, causation or
evidence, or some arithmetic operation — by its vowel.
{čur} are spacetime postpositions. They contain one of the
oral vowels |i|, |o|, or |ř|. Case postpositions are formed by
combining a {gun} with an appropriate {čur}, nearly always just {i,
o, ř}. Complex spacetime postpositions can include an epenthetic
schwa (ě).
{Φyr} are suffixes. They contain one of the vowels |a| or |ô|.
They become nasal if the suffix attaches to a root that
contains nasal vowels.
Verbs
There are four basic verb forms marked by suffixes applied to a noun
root (usually a root signifying an action, process, state, or quality).
van | stative (state, role, quality, non-agentive process) |
zô | active (deliberate, agentive process/action) |
ca | reflexive (agent acting upon itself) |
môj | reciprocal (agents acting on each other) |
Examples of all verb forms with the same noun-root: {bly} "falling,
orbit, throwing": (Glossing abbreviations)
bly-van.
|
fall-V.STATE
|
I'm falling.
|
bly-ca.
|
throw-V.REFL
|
I'm jumping.
|
bly-môj | pe | bly-θaj-môj | bly-ķĭm-tla | tu-i. |
throw-V.RECP | and | throw-OPP1-V.RECP | throw/jump-exercise-professional | AGT-at |
The acrobats throw and catch each other.
|
{Ќ} "I, me" is the default agent, experiencer or
topic, so it isn't expressed explicitly in the first three examples
above.
Time, aspect, mood, etc. are optionally shown with modifier particles
following the verb, such as
mwe | optative, imperative, hortative, jussive |
źǒ | negative imperative/optative |
še | maybe [facts] |
be | maybe [intentions] |
mje | past |
ler | future |
de | nowadays; lately; (with {mje}) in those days (habitual aspect, extended tense) |
If a temporal complement specifies a particular time when the action of
the sentence takes place, {mje} or {ler} is usually unnecessary.
dâ-ŋla | i | sâŋ | cǒ | ĥy-i | ķârm-zô. |
three-ORD.D | on | blood | few | PAT-at | cough-V.ACT. |
On Tuesday I coughed up a little blood.
|
Or if the time of a narrative has been specified by a postpositional
phrase like the above in one clause, one can generally infer that each
subsequent clause's action takes place a little after that of the
last, and again {mje} is not needed except to show relative
time, that this clause's action takes place before the time of the
surrounding narrative; in this use (its most common use in modern gzb)
it's more like a perfect aspect marker than a past tense marker.
(Early on, I used {mje} and {ler} a lot more often.)
A day of the week mentioned generally refers to the past instance of that
day, unless the next such instance is specified by {ler}:
Verb argument structures and the case postpositions
Verb arguments can be marked in five ways:
- a noun or pronoun phrase marked with a postposition
- a subordinate clause marked by a conjunction
- implicit, an omitted default subject
- subject pronoun incorporated into the verb
- subject in clause-final position with no postposition
Within the first two types, there are many specific postpositions and
a handful of subordinating conjunctions which can mark verb arguments.
Case-like postpositions can be derived from almost
any root word followed by one of the three basic spatial postpositions
(most commonly {i}, "at, in").
These are some of the case-like postpositions used most frequently.
tu-i | agent |
ĥy-i | patient (object affected by action) |
kâ-i | object of attention |
ʝâr-i | experiencer |
mĭ-i | topic |
ŋĭn-i | comment |
jâ-i | in such a state |
jâ-o | becoming |
jâ-ř | ceasing to be, changing from |
In active sentences, {-zô} marks the verb and {tu-i} and
{ĥy-i} typically mark the agent and patient. These are not the
same as subject and object in English and other Indo-European
languages; there is no passive voice for verbs. {tu-i} always denotes
an animate being who is intentionally doing something. {ĥy-i}
always denotes something that is affected by the action of the verb.
Some of the uses of the passive (e.g., saying that something
happens without saying who does it) can be rendered by use of
{mĭ-i} and {jâ-o}.
bĭm | ĥy-i | šyj-zô | ƥ | tu-i. |
tub | PAT-at | clean-V.ACT | 3 | AGT-at |
He cleans the tub. | |
bĭm | mĭ-i | šyj-bô | jâ-o. |
tub | TOP-at | clean-ADJ | state-to |
The tub becomes clean. |
If the object of the verb is not really acted upon in some
way by the agent, another role marker is used for it: for instance,
{mĭ-i} or {kâ-i} for object of thought or attention:
ť | kâ-i | rĭm-van.
|
2 | ATT-at | see-V.STATE
|
I see you.
|
ljâw-gjâ | mĭ-i | zym-zô. |
observational.science-language | TOP-at | think-V.ACT |
I think about linguistics. |
If the subject is not actively, intentionally doing something, then it
is typically marked with {ʝâr-i} "experiencer" (if
animate) or {mĭ-i} "topic" (if inanimate, or if the
semantics of the verb are not consistent with experiencer marking).
šî'fy | mâ-dân | kâ-i | ku-van | de | kâ'θij-ram | ʝâr-i. |
spirit | person-formerly | ATT-at | hear-V.STATE | HAB | Cathy-NAME | EXP-at |
Cathy hears ghosts.
|
ij'mâks-gam | mĭ-i | sjum-van | terij-ram | ʝâr-i. |
Emacs-NAME.G | TOP-at | thankful-V.STATE | Terry-NAME | EXP-at |
Terry is grateful for Emacs.
|
{ŋâw-o} is used for the object (addressee or listener) of
a communication-verb:
dejv-ram | ŋâw-o | twâ-zô | Φǒ | {ť | hǒ}. |
Dave-NAME | call-to | say-V.ACT | QUOTE | 2 | VOC |
I said "Hey!" to Dave. |
If the object of the verb didn't already exist,
but is created by the action, it's marked by
{ķĭn-o} (being constructed, put together from physical materials),
{krĭ-o} (being thought up, written, composed, etc.),
or {bĭŋ-o} (coming into existence).
gjâ | o-m | gun | krĭ-o | zym-zô | krĭ-gjâ-pja | tu-i. |
language | to-part.of | root.word | create-to | think-V.ACT | create-language-amateur | AGT-at |
The conlanger thinks up words for [into] the language.
|
gî'bu | i | θě'ku | tu-i | krĭ-zô | fî'suň | pe | mu | ble | bĭŋ-o. |
beginning | at | God | AGT-at | create-V.ACT | Earth | and | universe | rest.of | existence-to. |
In the beginning God created the heavens and the earth. |
The topic of a topic-comment sentence and the "object" of
some verbs of thinking and feeling are marked the same way, with the
postposition {mĭ-i}, which was translated in some sentences above
as "about".
râm | mĭ-i | pâ-źa-bô
| ŋĭn-i. |
cat | TOP-at | restless-AUG-ADJ |
CMT-at |
The cat is hyperactive.
|
râm | mĭ-i | zym-zô. |
cat | TOP-at | think-V.ACT |
I'm thinking about the cat.
râm | tu-i | zym-zô. |
cat | AGT-at | think-V.ACT |
The cat is thinking [about something unspecified].
râm | kâ-i | rĭm-van. |
cat | ATT-at | see-V.STATE |
I see the cat.
źum-la-zô | râm | ĥy-i. |
touch-AFF-V.ACT | cat | PAT-at |
I stroke the cat.
Some other theta-role postpositions:
ĉul-i | transitory object of performance
|
rjâ-i | object of quest or desire
|
čĭ-ř | copied from, extracted from, quoted from
|
θĭ-o | on behalf of, for the benefit of
|
θĭ-ř | by/with the help of
|
ĥun-i | with, together with, in company with
|
pě'ŝlĭ-i | focused body part or mental faculty
|
jĭrn-i | quantity, measure
|
{ĉul-i} is used for the direct objects of verbs like "sing", "read aloud", "perform",
and "play": transitory processes that are over when the action of the verb is over,
or which are the action of the verb re-conceptualized as nouns, or transitory
process-instantiations of more permanent abstract informational entities.
kuln |
reŋ |
ĥun-i |
{oklahomě-wam-ŋa} |
ĉul-i |
hwâwm-ŝrun-zô |
ƥ |
tu-i. |
friend |
many |
meeting-at |
Oklahoma-NAME.P-ATD.surprise |
perform-at |
acting-music-V.ACT |
3 |
AGT-at |
She performed/sang in "Oklahoma!" with several friends. |
go-gam |
ĉul-i |
pĭw-môj |
Ќ |
ke |
ĭnza-ram. |
Go-NAME.G |
perform-at |
play-V.RECP |
1 |
and |
Ynza-NAME |
Ynza and I played Go. |
{rjâ-i}, object of quest, is used for the objects of verbs of
searching, desiring, requesting, etc., that are not necessarily
present or even existent, in contrast to {kâ-i}, object of attention,
where the object is generally within range of the experiencer's
senses.
{Codex Seraphinianus} |
rjâ-i |
sru-θô-van. |
(title) |
quest-at |
desire-DIM-V.STATE |
I would like a copy of the Codex Seraphinianus (but am not likely to get it). |
mǒj |
te-ta-ƥ-van. |
but |
3.INAN-without-3-V.STATE |
I asked the clerk for some postage stamps, too; but she had none. |
{θĭ} "helping" derives two different benefactive postpositions:
ĭnza-ram |
θĭ-ř |
gjâ-krĭ |
pǒ |
kujm-o |
Φyr-tôn-cu |
krĭ-o. |
Ynza-NAME |
help-from |
language-create |
that |
purpose-to |
suffix-GNR-system |
create-to |
With Ynza's help I created an inflection paradigm for that conlang. |
{pě'ŝlĭ} signifies the body part or mental faculty one's attention
is currently focused upon, or which is clamoring for attention. The
postposition {pě'ŝlĭ-i} is used in place of {kâ-i} or {mĭ-i} for
body parts in some contexts.
tâlm |
pě'ŝlĭ-i |
jyn-cô-van. |
head |
focal.part-at |
pleasure-OPP2-V.STATE |
My head hurts. / I have a headache. = lit., I suffer pain focused in my head. |
vâ |
pě'ŝlĭ-i |
žâw-van |
plyn-bô. |
digestion |
focal.part-at |
feel-V.STATE |
full-ADJ |
I feel full / I don't feel hungry. / lit., I feel fullness focused in the digestive system. |
{jĭrn-i} marks a measured quantity that normally applies to the
action of the main verb; for instance,
θuň |
pǒ |
krĭ-o |
lju-θaj-zô. |
story |
DEM3 |
create-to |
read-OPP1-V.ACT |
I wrote a thousand words of that story today. |
Polysemy of {mĭ-i}
{mĭ-i} can mark different semantic roles depending on the particular
verb or the other postpositions it's used with. This was not
originally intended; it's one of the aspects of the language that
developed from use
rather than being deliberately designed.
When I find that some part of the language is more
complex or irregular than I intended, I don't necessarily change it;
if I've already learned to use it fairly fluently, I leave it alone
and just try to document the way I find that I actually use it.
The original design of gzb's case-postposition system was influenced
indirectly by Japanese and similar
topic-prominent languages
— not directly by a serious study of the language,
but by a few example sentences and a brief discussion in a linguistics
textbook. It was also influenced by my then-misunderstanding of the
linguistics term "topic", used in various senses by different schools
of linguists and in different branches of linguistics. The result
of these misunderstandings is that {mĭ-i}, though I've customarily
glossed it as the "topic postposition", has four or five main senses,
only one of which even roughly corresponds to the
topic marker
of a true topic-prominent language like Japanese.
- {mĭ-i} is used with the "comment" postposition {ŋĭn-i} in verbless
sentences for simple
predication,
and similarly with the state and state-transition postpositions
{jâ-i, jâ-o, jâ-ř}. Here, it corresponds with the subject of English
copular sentences (with "to be" or "to become").
- With verbs of thinking, feeling, and communication,
it marks the thing about which the
agent or experiencer is thinking, feeling or talking, corresponding to English
direct object or oblique object depending on the syntax of the
particular English verb.
- It's also used to mark the inanimate
subjects of many stative verbs, in contrast with the experiencer
postposition {ʝâr-i} for animate subjects of stative verbs.
- And for some stative verbs, such as locatives,
it can mark animate subjects as well.
- It can work as a kind of genitive postposition, e.g. marking
what a story is about or what is portrayed in a picture, in which
case it binds to the following noun rather than to the main
verb or the sentence structure as a whole; here it corresponds
to some senses of the English prepositions "of" or "about".
-
Historically, it used to mark the
object of attention for verbs of perception, and the animate subjects
of all stative verbs; but few if any of the texts on this website use it
in those archaic senses.
Predication with quality/state term:
Predication with quantity:
gĭ |
jĭrn-i |
θĭl |
mĭ-i. |
eleven |
amount-at |
potato |
TOP-at |
There are eleven potatoes (here). |
Predication with entity term:
ʝâr-ku-zym |
ŋĭn-i |
dejvĭd-ram |
mĭ-i. |
experiencer-hear-thought |
CMT-at |
David-NAME |
TOP-at |
David is a passive telepath. |
Predication of companionship:
With verb of thinking:
mluj |
krĭ-gjâ-za |
mĭ-i |
zym-gâw-zô. |
small.conference |
create-language-ADJ2 |
TOP-at |
think-plan-V.ACT |
I'm planning for the [next] Language Creation Conference. |
With emotion verb:
With communication verb:
takahiro-ram |
ŋâw-o |
flyr-ĵwa |
mĭ-i |
gju-ť-zô |
mwe. |
Takahiro-NAME |
call-to |
flower-place |
TOP-at |
speak-2-V.ACT |
IMP |
Talk to Takahiro about the garden. |
As topical genitive:
plutarĥǒs-ram |
dâm-ř |
lě'kjân |
mĭ-i |
twâ-cu |
kâ-i |
lju-zô. |
Plutarch-NAME |
authorship-from |
Alexander |
TOP-at |
sentence-system |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
I'm reading Plutarch's life of Alexander. |
hĭnrij-ram |
ðy-dâ-pa |
mĭ-i |
gâm |
krĭ-o |
ķĭn-zô |
hanz-ram |
holbin-šam |
ĵĭn-sra-bô |
tu-i. |
Henry-NAME |
five-three-ORD |
TOP-at |
picture |
create-to |
construct-V.ACT |
Hans-NAME |
Holbein-NAME.F |
young-CMP-ADJ |
AGT-at |
Hans Holbein the Younger painted a portrait of Henry VIII. |
Inanimate subject of stative verb:
Animate subject of locative verb:
Argument structure classes
In the lexicon, many verb entries (though far from all, unfortunately)
contain an explanation of the verb's argument structure, like this:
žy-zô | to show, point out (person {tu-i} shows thing {kâ-i} to person {ĥy-i})
|
ĵulm-van | to deserve, to merit; {mĭ-i} deserves {ðĭ-i}
|
In the table below, these verbs' structures are indicated as "kâ-i
~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i" or "ðĭ-i ~~~ mĭ-i". A few other notes: {o/ř}
represents any motion postposition, and {i} represents any stationary
locative postposition. The arguments are listed in the order they
would typically appear in a sentence, with the subject at the end.
Note that the argument structures of verbs aren't cast in stone; for
instance, many argument structures with a {mĭ-i} place can substitute
{ʝar-i} instead if the argument is animate, and often some or even
all of the arguments can be omitted. Temporal and locative
complements can usually be added to almost any verb; {i} or {o/ř} are
listed as arguments below only if they often occur with a certain
class of verbs.
Argument structure | Typical verbs
|
mĭ-i | bly-van, flâň-van
|
tu-i | ķĭm-ca, pjylm-syl-môj
|
ʝâr-i | huw-van, fĭm-van
|
ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | ĥâ-zô, vâ-oŋ-zô, šâ-zô, tru-zô
|
kâ-i ~~~ tu-i | kâ-rĭm-zô, lju-zô, suŋ-hôw-ca
|
rjâ-i ~~~ tu-i | rě'ju-zô
|
mĭ-i ~~~ tu-i | zym-zô
|
mĭ-i ~~~ ʝâr-i | sjum-van, blâl-van
|
kâ-i ~~~ ʝâr-i | rĭm-van, ku-van, lym-van
|
rjâ-i ~~~ ʝâr-i | sru-van
|
ðĭ-i ~~~ mĭ-i | slân-van, wuŋ-van, ĵulm-van
|
krĭ-o ~~~ tu-i | lju-θaj-zô, gâm-zô
|
ķĭn-o ~~~ tu-i | vâ-oŋ-faj-fwa-zô
|
kâ-ř ~~~ tu-i | zym-ʝǒ-zô
|
ĥun-i ~~~ mĭ-i | gju-môj, ĝu-ðĭl-môj
|
pě'ŝlĭ-i ~~~ ʝâr-i | jyn-cô-van, žâw-van
|
kâ-i ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | žy-zô, ku-fwa-zô
|
(ŝâj-o) ~~~~ (ŝâj-ř) ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | kâj-zô
|
ŝâj-o ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | bwĭl-zô, ₣yjm-zô
|
ŝâj-ř ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | tâň-zô
|
ŝâj-ř ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ ʝâr-i | bwĭl-θaj-van
|
o/ř ~~~ tu-i | ruŋ-zô, ƴâ-zô, zyŋ-zô
|
o/ř ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | fyn-zô, ₣yjm-zô, čuj-zô
|
o/ř ~~~ mĭ-i | flu-van
|
i ~~~ mĭ-i | tyn-van, ty-van, su-van
|
ŋâw-o ~~~ mĭ-i / hǒŋ / Φǒ / mĭ-šar ~~~ tu-i | twâ-zô, gju-zô, grâm-zô
|
ŋâw-o ~~~ rjâ-i / hǒŋ ~~~ tu-i | lâ-zô
|
ĉul-i ~~~ tu-i | pĭw-zô, ŝrun-zô, lju-gju-zô
|
ŋâw-o ~~~ čĭ-ř ~~~ tu-i | grâm-zô, ĵĭ-zô
|
ŋĭn-i ~~~ mĭ-i ~~~ tu-i | kĭ-zô, zym-zô, twâ-zô
|
(jâ-o) ~~~ (jâ-ř) ~~~ ĥy-i ~~~ tu-i | ₣âl-zô, ĉâm-zô
|
ðǒŋ | mĭ-van, nî'sâ-van, le-tǒj-van
|
hǒŋ ~~~ tu-i | zym-zô
|
hǒŋ ~~~ ʝâr-i | hyw-van, gwě'vu-van
|
This list is still incomplete, and I'm not sure there's not a simpler
way to analyze verbs' arguments structures or a more concise way to
present them.
Volitionality
{-van} verbs are not necessarily intransitive, and {-zô} verbs
are not necessarily transitive. The distinction is partly between
nonvolitional and volitional, and partly between static and dynamic.
{-zô} verbs always imply an animate agent. An agentive, dynamic
process will always be denoted with a {zô}-verb, unless it's
reflexive or reciprocal, and an agentless state will be denoted by a
{van} verb (if by a verb at all; or possibly by a postpositional
phrase or an adjective). Agentless processes (such as involuntary
acts like breathing and seeing, or "acts" of inanimate
objects like water flowing) are also denoted by {-van} verbs. I
haven't yet worked out the detailed rules for handling agentive states
(if it even makes sense to speak of such).
Changes in argument structure with opposite-suffixes
The opposite-suffixes
{-θaj} and {-cô}
have a more or less predictable effect on the argument
structures of verbs. With prototypical
ditransitive verbs
such as {bwĭl-zô}, "to give",
{-θaj} makes the source or recipient of the basic verb the
experiencer of a derived verb, leaving the patient the same, and
allowing the agent to be omitted, while {-cô} reverses the
action of the verb and leaves its argument structure the same.
With verbs of perception, {-θaj} makes the object-of-attention of
the stem verb into the topic of the derived verb, and allows
the experiencer to be omitted.
Changes in argument structure with causative suffixes
The causative suffixes {-fwa} and {-hôw} are discussed at greater length in the derivational morphology document.
They can be used to derive either modifiers or verbs. Here, I'll briefly describe how the syntax of a verb changes when it becomes causative.
If a verb normally has an experiencer argument and an object of attention argument, making it causative makes the experiencer into a patient, and adds an agent argument who is the one that is causing the patient to undergo the action of the verb. The object of attention argument remains the same. E.g.:
A verb of thinking, feeling or knowing, which typically has a topic argument (with {mĭ-i}) or sometimes an object of attention argument, along with an experiencer or agent argument depending on the activeness of the verb, acts in a similar way. The topic or object of attention argument remains unchanged while the experiencer or agent becomes a patient, and a new agent argument appears for the causer.
Sometimes the object of attention or topic in a causative of feeling is omitted, in which case we can pragmatically assume that the causer is trying to make the causee feel in such a way about the causer themselves. E.g.:
However, if the verb normally takes as patient argument, the above process would
result in two patient arguments, it being unclear (especially if both are of the
same animacy) which is being caused to do something and which is having
something done to them. Some languages allow this with their causatives, but in
gzb, the original patient argument becomes a generic {ðĭ-i} argument,
as follows:
Note that in all these causatives, as well as in lexical causatives like
{žy-zô} "to show" (i.e. "cause to see"), the
intermediate cause is always marked as the patient, and the causative verb
always takes the {-zô} active verb ending.
Argument structures of verbless sentences
Not only topic-comment and experiencer-state structures allow
verbless sentences; several of the specific object postpositions
allow verbless agentive sentences, with the verb implicit in
the semantics of the object and the postposition.
{ķĭn-o} in particular is likely to occur in verbless clauses; in my
electronic corpus, it occurs with a verb only four times, versus nine
times in verbless clauses.
Default arguments of verbs
As noted before, {Ќ} (I, me) is the default
topic/experiencer/agent of a sentence. This applies at
the beginning of a text or conversation, when there is no previous
context.
lju-zô.
read-V.ACT
I read, am reading.
kě'pâ-van.
happy.confusion-V.STATE
I'm happily confused.
When there is previous context, however, the default topic,
experiencer or agent of each sentence is the same entity as
was last explicitly marked as topic, experiencer, or agent. (This also
goes for subordinate clauses, whose subject defaults to be the same
as in the main clause.)
kâj-kô | o | ruŋ-zô | tam-ram | tu-i. |
exchange-place | to | go-V.ACT | Tom-NAME | AGT-at |
re | i | gâ-rjâ | ĥy-i | tru-zô | heŋ. |
3.PLACE | at | thing-quest | PAT-at | find-V.ACT | not |
Tom went to the store. [He] didn't find what he was looking for there.
A vocative phrase sets the default subject, as well:
?najĝěl-ram |
hǒ, |
râm |
ðĭ-i |
ħě'nâw-van |
zǒn. |
Nigel-NAME |
VOC |
cat |
relationship-at |
allergy-V.STATE |
Q.YN |
Nigel, are you allergic to cats? |
This means that agentless processes and states have to be expressed
otherwise than with subjectless verbs, as in Esperanto's
"pluvas", "necesas ke...". If I translated those
literally with just a bare verb, the result would mean "I rain, I
am necessary that...", or depending on the context, it might
attribute these actions to any random entity that was recently
mentioned. Nor do I use a dummy subject as in English "It's
raining".
bly-van | pwĭm | mĭ-i |
fall-V.STATE | water | TOP-at |
Water is falling = It's raining.
jâln-van | purj | mĭ-i. |
hot-V.STATE | environment | TOP-at |
It's hot.
If the subject of the sentence comes last (as it usually does)
then the final postposition (agent, experiencer, or topic)
may be omitted.
žuln-van | byn-pja. |
satisfaction.with.work-V.STATE | hack-amateur |
The hacker is pleased with his work.
This applies also if the subject of a clause is the last thing in
that clause, even if it's not the end of the sentence; for instance,
{dě'dâ-lô} immediately precedes the quote-conjunction {Φǒ}, so it
needn't be explicitly marked as subject (in this case, agent) of
{twâ-zô}. The same is true for subjects preceding other
clause-scope conjunctions, such as
{kiň}
"and", {hǒŋ}
"that", and
{vĭj-šar}
"then, next".
Ditransitive verbs
In Indo-European languages, the term "ditransitive" is used for verbs
that are pretty much required to have both a direct and an indirect
object. I use the same term for verbs in gzb that usually if not
always occur with two different objects, though with gzb's theta-role
marking system, it's not always easy to say which of the core
arguments for a given gzb ditransitive would correspond to
a direct vs. an indirect object in another language; and gzb, like
English, is prone to dropping arguments that are obvious from context
(e.g., "Give it here" where the theoretically required "to me"
indirect object argument is omitted).
Probably the prototypical ditransitive verb in most languages is the
equivalent of "give": gzb {bwĭl-zô},
Here the gift is marked with {ĥy-i}, patient, and the recipient of the gift is marked
with {ŝâj-o}, coming-into-the-possession-of. Straightforward enough. The
verb {kâj-zô}
"buy/sell/trade"
has a similar argument structure, but can have additional arguments.
gâm | prym-fwa | kâ-i | Ќ | ĥy-i | žy-zô | krĭ-gâm-tla | pǒ. |
picture | aesthetic.appreciation-CAUS | ATT-at | 1 | PAT-at | show-V.ACT | create-picture-professional | DEM3 |
That artist showed me a beautiful picture. |
Here, the person being shown something is the patient {ĥy-i}, and
the thing being shown gets the attentive case postposition,
{kâ-i}. The simpler English equivalent is ambiguous (in how one
would analyze the cases of "me" and "a beautiful
picture", I mean, not in what it means), but the more formal
version would be "...showed a beautiful
picture to me", i.e. the picture is the direct
object; and the same would be true in the other Indo-European
languages I'm familiar with.
The thinking verb {kĭ-zô} "to deem / consider X to have quality Y"
adds an agent to what would otherwise be a simple topic-comment
sentence. E.g.,
ħâl-fwa | ŋĭn-i | ₣â | pǒ | mĭ-i. |
nervous.fear-CAUS | CMT-at | task | DEM3 | TOP-at |
This task induces the jitters. |
In other words, the "direct" and "indirect" objects of {kĭ-zô} and
similar verbs are marked as topic and comment. ({kĭ-zô} also has an
alternate argument structure, where it takes an object subordinate
clause marked with {hǒŋ}.)
The verb {ðĭl-zô}, "to type or transcribe", is potentially
ditransitive in the second sense:
twâ-cu-hân | kǒ | lju-i | pě'pâ-ga | om | ðĭl-Ќ-zô. |
sentence-system-old | DEM1 | read-at | page-METAPH | into | type-1-V.ACT |
I'm transcribing this old book into an electronic document. |
Here the objects are marked as {lju-i}, a kind of performative case
more specific than {ĉul-i}, and {om}, becoming-part-of. However, I'm
not sure {ðĭl-zô} actually qualifies as ditransitive since the
{om} argument is optional.
The opposite-suffix {-θaj} as used with some stems that form
ditransitive verbs makes the source/recipient of the basic verb the
experiencer of a derived verb, leaving the patient the same, and
allowing the agent to be omitted.
Giving and receiving are one process; {bwĭl-zô} and {bwĭl-θaj-van}
simply focus on different aspects of it from different participants'
perspective. The other opposite-suffix {-cô} does not necessarily
affect the theta-roles of the participants in the action of the verb,
but it changes the meaning of the underlying action:
Reflexive and reciprocal verbs
A reflexive verb can occur with an explicit patient, topic or attentive
postpositional phrase; usually this signifies a body part or faculty of the
agent or experiencer, e.g.:
!maŋ | ĥy-i | šyj-ca | mwe | ť. |
hand | PAT-at | clean-V.REFL | IMP | 2 |
Wash your hands.
Ќ | im | tâlm | vin | kâ-i | rĭm-ca-ĉa | syj-i | rĭm-ca. |
1 | part.of | head | front-surface | ATT-at | see-V.REFL-tool | use-at | see-V.REFL |
I see my face in [using] the mirror.
gě'dĭm | pen | šin | žâj-ŋĭw | kâ-i | byn-ca | mwe | Ł. |
sleep.wake.cycle | every | end-of | moral.law-faculty | ATT-at | poke.around-V.REFL | IMP | 3.GEN |
One should examine one's conscience every night.
Because gjâ-zym-byn does not have a sharp distinction between direct
objects and oblique objects, the reflexive and reciprocal verbs formed
with {-ca} and {-môj} sometimes have as their reflexive objects
things which would be expressed with oblique objects or complements in
other languages. A few verbs tend to almost always take {-môj} when
the subject is plural.
hyr | srǒ | il | gju-môj | tam-ram | pe | ser'ě-ram. |
hour | several | during | speak-V.REFL | Tom-NAME | and | Sarah-NAME |
Tom and Sarah talked [with each other] for hours.
Logically, perhaps, {gju-môj} ought to mean "to talk
about each other". But one of my design principles for
gjâ-zym-byn is not to change something if I've already learned to
use it fluently, just because I later decide it's not perfectly
logical. I did not design any irregularity into the language
deliberately, but since my goal is to learn to use it fluently myself,
and not to devise a language that's easy for people in general to
learn, I'm perfectly happy with keeping any irregularity that creeps
into the language through my occasional carelessness, if I don't
notice it's irregular until I've already learned it.
Sequential verbs
Where the typical Indo-European language would use an auxiliary verb followed by
a particple or infinitive, gjâ-zym-byn just uses two verbs in sequence.
The first verb roughly corresponds to an auxiliary in English or French and the
second verb roughly corresponds to an infinitive, though it gets no special
marking. Either verb can be marked with {-van} or {-zô} according to its
meaning.
âθ'ĭnz-wam | o | sru-van | ruŋ-zô. |
Athens-NAME.P | to | want-V.STATE | go-V.ACT |
I want to go to Athens. [= Athens, Georgia; the Greek one is
{a'θen'aj'ǒs-wam}, the one in Kentucky is {ej'θĭnz-wam}.]
dlu-van |
heŋ |
huw-Ł-van. |
right-V.STATE |
NEG |
happy-3.GEN-V.STATE |
One doesn't have a right to be happy. |
mǒj | dlu-van | vǒm | rjâ-zô | huw-van. |
but | right-V.STATE | yes | seek-V.ACT | happy-V.STATE |
But one does have a right to seek to be happy.
Note that modifiers to the main verb can come between it and the second verb,
as with {heŋ} above.
Sometimes the first of a sequence of verbs is not a typical
auxiliary verb.
mî'ħâ-van | krĭ-šĭm-zô | byn-pja. |
obsession-V.STATE | create-algorithm-V.ACT | hack-amateur |
The hacker is obsessively coding. |
Sometimes arguments of the second verb in a sequence come
between the two verbs:
But if there are multiple arguments to the verb, or one argument with a lot of
words, it's probably better style/less confusing to put the second verb and its
arguments in a subordinate clause (with the appropriate conjunction for the main
verb's meaning) or relative clause, e.g.,
Subject Pronoun Incorporation
gjâ-zym-byn can optionally incorporate a subject pronoun
into the verb; it affixes between the verb stem and the verb suffix. For
serial verbs, the pronoun will generally only be incorporated into the
first of the series.
prym-fwa-ť-van. |
appreciation.of.beauty-CAUS-2-V.STATE |
You're beautiful. |
twâ-cu | pǒ | kâ-i | vy-ƥ-van | lju-zô. |
sentence-system | DEM3 | ATT-at | intend-3-V.STATE | read-V.ACT |
She intends to read that book. |
Such pronoun incorporation is usually done only when
-
there is no explicit subject noun phrase, and
-
the correct subject cannot be inferred by the default subject rule
hǒŋ | fĭm-cô-ť-van. |
that | healthy-OPP2-2-V.STATE |
I talked with Kelly. She told me you were sick. |
First-person plural subject attraction
In gzb, sometimes a comitative phrase expressed with {ĥun-i}, "with",
will influence the verb form and/or the subject, making the verb
reciprocal or the subject plural, thus:
English would express these as "I played chess with Val" or "I went
there with Tom."
Aspect
gjâ-zym-byn does not have grammatical category of aspect as
such, but several aspectual distinctions are commonly
marked by affixes or by root words compounded into
verbs.
Cessative/Perfect:
ť | dâm-ř | grâm | kâ-i | lju-sun-zô | mje | θǒ. |
2 | authorship-from | message | ATT-at | read-finish-V.ACT | past | immediate |
I've just finished reading your letter. |
Progressive:
vâ-oŋ-vĭj-zô | ƥ. |
digestion-into-time.period-V.ACT | 3 |
He goes on eating. |
Inceptive:
pĭw-gĭn-zô | θǒ | Ќ-ƥ, | nu-šar | vě'ty-θaj | ĥy-i |
trâw-zô | mâ | kwǒ. |
play-begin-V.ACT | immediate | 1-3 | moment-CONJ | doorway-OPP | PAT-at |
strike-V.ACT | person | some |
We had just started playing when someone knocked at
the door. |
Iterative:
kyl-pwĭm-daj | rol-lol | čâ-ra-zô | lu'ĭs-ram. |
box-water-mass | across.through-hither.through | swim-repeat-V.ACT | Louis-NAME |
Louis swam [laps] across the pool several times. |
Punctual/Semelfactive:
ķarm-nu-zô | ku-faj-źa | râm. |
cough-moment-V.ACT | hear-able-AUG | cat |
The cat coughed once loudly. |
The adverb {de} already mentioned marks a habitual aspect.
tâŋ | pǒ | i, | nĭvĭn-šam | dâm-ř | θuň | reŋ | kâ-i | lju-zô | de. |
life.period | DEM3 | at | Niven-NAME.F | authorship-from | story | many | ATT-at | read-V.ACT | HAB |
I was reading a lot of stories by Niven in those days. |
{de} can also mark e.g. the day of the
week when something is regularly done or regularly happens.
See also the section on the
qualifier {jǒm} "most of,
mostly" for examples of its aspectual use with verbs.
Gerunds and Participles
There is no need for special morphology to mark gerunds, since the
root words for actions, events and processes are already nominal.
ty | oŋ | ruŋ | š-i-j, | vâ-oŋ-zô. |
home | into | going | after-at-near | digestion-into-V.ACT. |
Soon after coming home, I ate. |
hwâwm | mĭ-i | suŋ-hôw-zô | rěbekě-ram | tu-i. |
acting | TOP-at | know.how-CAUS2-V.ACT | Rebecca-NAME | AGT-at |
Rebecca teaches acting. |
When a verb is derived indirectly (e.g. from a postpositional phrase),
there is no root noun that means the same thing as the verb, so one can
use the nominalizer clitic {tǒj} to obtain such a gerund:
ĥun-pĭw | ðij | vâ-oŋ-tǒj | mĭ-i | gâw-zô, | mǒj | ce | heŋ. |
meeting-play | before | digestion-into-NMZ | TOP-at | consider-V.ACT | but | this | not. |
I considered eating before the party, but decided not to. |
Any direct object must immediately precede the gerund; the verb or
comment on the gerund clause usually comes after the gerund.
₣ĭŋ | kyl-plâŋ-za | ĥy-i | lĭn | žu-bô | mĭ-i | hum-ga-van. |
string | box-foot-ADJ2 | PAT-at | linking | careful-ADJ | TOP-at | deep-MET-V.STATE |
It's important to tie [one's] shoestrings carefully. |
Use {tu} "agent" and {ĥy} "patient" to form nominal participles.
lju | act or process of reading |
lju-zô | to read; I read, he reads, ... |
tu-lju | reader; person reading |
ĥy-lju | the thing read |
Note that this use of {ĥy} is not entirely consistent with the
way the verb {lju-zô} is used. Reading may affect the physical
book {twâ-cu-vuj} (in terms of slight wear and tear) but it does
not affect the text of the book {twâ-cu} (abstracted from its
instantiation in particular printed copies). So normally one would
use the attentive case postposition instead of the patient case:
twâ-cu | ĵyn-fwa | kâ-i | lju-zô. |
sentence-system | interest-CAUS | ATT-at | read-V.ACT |
I'm reading an interesting book. |
twâ-cu-vuj | hân-bô | nâ-cô-bô | ĥy-i | lju-zô | žu-bô | mwe | Ł. |
sentence-system-physical | old-ADJ | common-OPP2-ADJ | PAT-at | read-V.ACT | careful-ADJ | IMP | 3.GEN |
One must read rare old books carefully. |
The first form (with {kâ-i}) emphasizes the content of the book
(and doesn't specify its format, whether it is printed, an etext, or
even an audiobook). The second, with {ĥy-i}, emphasizes the
physical act of handling the book, turning the pages. The first is by
far the more common way of marking the "direct object" of
{lju-zô}. So does {ĥy-lju} refer mainly to a physical book,
magazine, etc.? Not necessarily. {kâ-lju} would mean something
very different: "reading attention", or "act of
attention characterized in some way by reading". So it could not
refer to the "thing read" in the sense of the content of a
book as distinct from its embodiment in a particular copy of a
particular edition. Therefore {ĥy-lju} has to do double duty for
both senses, and in short {ĥy} is not so specific when acting as
a participle base as when it is acting as a postposition base. (If
necessary, one can be more specific by referring to {ĥy-lju-vuj},
physical thing read, or {ĥy-lju-vuj-cô}, abstract thing
read.)
One can add {-bô} to these nominal participles to form modifer
participles:
ƴâw-bâm | tu-pĭw-bô | kâ-i | pym-van. |
dog-new | AGT-play-ADJ | ATT-at | amusement-V.STATE |
I'm amused at the puppy playing.
rjuŋ | kâ-i | ħun-tôn-daj | kiŋ | tru-zô | tu-pĭw-bô | mâ-ĵĭn. |
dragon | ATT-at | pine.tree-GNR-mass | among | find-V.ACT | AGT-play-ADJ | person-young |
The children found a dragon [while] playing in the forest.
ĥy-tru | v-ř | ruŋ-zô | ĵwy-bô. |
PAT-find | front-from | go-V.ACT | fast-ADJ |
They ran away from what they found.
Experiencer participles
The root word {ʝâr}, "experiencer", also forms a kind of participles.
ʝâr-pym | one who is experiencing amusement |
ʝâr-fĭm-cô | one who is experiencing sickness; a sick person or animal |
ʝâr-bly | one who is falling |
ʝâr-ħĭn | one who is experiencing restrictions; a prisoner |
ĝyl-fyn | mĭ-i | ʝâr-rĭm | dâ-bô | ĥy-i | tru-zô | Ќ-ɱ | gwe. |
interruption-drive | TOP-at | experiencer-see | three-ADJ | PAT-at | find-V.ACT | 1-3 | already |
We've found three witnesses to the accident so far. |
Postpostions
The core postpositions are:
i | at, in, near, with; during |
ř | from, out of; since |
o | to, toward; until |
One can make them more specific with various other single-phoneme
morphemes prefixed (for orientation) or suffixed (for proximity).
These prefixes and suffixes occur only with these core
spacetime postpositions, and never affix to any other morpheme.
Suffixes: being near, far or inside:
-m | in (part of) |
-ŋ | in (contained by) |
-n | touching the outside of |
-j | near |
-r | far from |
-l | through, throughout, all through |
So, for instance,
iŋ | inside |
oŋ | into |
řŋ | out of |
im | part of |
oj | toward but not (yet) at |
ir | far from |
řl | through (coming this direction, toward the speaker) |
These morphemes show orientation about a center:
Relative:
v- | in front of |
h- | behind |
ĵ- | right |
c- | left |
k- | among, between |
ĉ- | all around, surrounding |
r- | at, to, from the other side of |
l- | at, to, from this side of |
s- | above |
θ- | below |
š- | after, later part of (time) |
ð- | before, early part of (time) |
Absolute:
b- | north |
ħ- | south |
ź- | west |
g- | east |
Example spacetime postpositions:
sij | above (not touching) |
sin | on (touching the surface of) |
siŋ | in the upper part of
(rî'mâ siŋ pě'pâ-daj, papers in the attic) |
sim | in the upper part of
(Ќ sim šĭm-ŋĭw, my brain) |
so | going above |
son | onto |
sřn | off of |
θij | under (not touching) |
θin | under (touching) |
θo | going under |
θř | from under |
θoŋ | into the lower part of |
ĵi | on the right side of |
ci | on the left side of |
vi | in front of |
hi | behind |
hiŋ | in the back part of |
vř | from in front |
vo | to in front |
kin | between (touching the things it's between, e.g. a bookmark between pages) |
kiŋ | throughout (mushrooms scattered through a forest) |
il | through (a road going through a forest) |
ol | through (a man walks through a forest) |
rir | far beyond |
ron | coming to touch the far side of |
li | on this side of |
ĉi | surrounding |
ĉoŋ | into from all sides |
źi | on the west side of |
ħř | from the south of |
i(ŋ) | during, while |
ši | after |
ði | before |
ðo(n) | until, up to |
šř(n) | since, from that time |
Complex directions can be specified by using two of the prefixes
and inserting an epenthetic schwa between them, thus:
běźir | far to the northwest of |
sěviŋ | in the upper front part of |
Uses of "before" and "after" postpositions {ði} and {ši}
These "before" and "after" postpositions (and their derivatives) are
used not only with nouns and noun phrases denoting time periods, but
with words for other things that are conceived of as having their
extension primarily in time rather than space.
tyn-van | ʝel, | pwiň | frâ | i-m | bu-kyr |
š-i-m | tyn-te-van. |
place-V.STATE | generally | or | question | at-part.of | phase-verb |
after-at-part.of | place-3.INAN-V.STATE |
The particle "zǒn" or
"srem" is placed at the end of a yes/no question, or at the end of
a verb phrase within the question.
|
mǒj | te | ĝy-i-m | tâň-van | ƥ. |
but | 3.INAN | middle-at-part.of | removal-V.STATE | 3 |
Several interesting characters appear in the early part of that story, but they disappear in the middle of it. |
A few of the 357 postpositions one can form in this system don't make
any sense. But most of them are potentially usable in some situation
or other. For instance, {šom}, "becoming part of the ending
of" could be used if one is talking about reforming a calendar
system and reassigning some days from the beginning of one month to
the end of the previous month, or, less farfetchedly,
mluj | š-o-m | ĥun-frâ | ĥy-i | tâň-θaj-Ł-zô. |
convention | end-to-part.of | meeting-question | PAT-at | take-OPP1-3GEN-V.ACT |
They added a question and answer session at the end of the convention. |
Postpositions in {ĉ-}
Most of the spacetime postpositions are fairly straightforward, but
those in {ĉ-} require some more explanation. "ĉi"
refers to a position surrounding the object on all sides,
"ĉo" to motion of something that begins to surround the
object. Neither refers to going around something, circumnavigating
it. The adverbs "ŝwe" (widdershins) and
"ŝwe-θaj" (clockwise) can be used together with
"ĉi" or "ĉo" to indicate such motion.
rîmâ-źa | ĉ-i | tyn-van | pwĭm-daj. |
house-AUG | around-at | place-V.STATE | water-mass |
There is a moat around the castle. |
rîmâ-źa | ĉ-o | ruŋ-zô | sî'ðyr-tla-cu. |
house-AUG | around-to | go-V.ACT | fight-professional-system |
The army surrounded the castle. |
rîmâ-ĵwa | jeriĥo-wam | ĉ-i | ŝwe | ƴâ-zô | fy-bô | jisrael-tam-cu. |
house-place | Jericho-NAME.P | around-at | widdershins | walk-V.ACT | seven-ADJ | Israel-NAME.E-system |
The Israelites marched around Jericho seven times. |
Serial postpositions
Sometimes two postpositions in a row are used, the second postposition
modifying the first.
swyŋ | s-i-n | ĥy-i | ĉârn-zô | šyj-zô. |
table | top-at-contact | PAT-at | abrade-V.ACT | clean-V.ACT |
I scrub the surface of the table. |
mruň | ħ-i-m | Φâ | kâ-i | kujm-o | re | o | ruŋ-zô. |
mountain | south-at-part.of | form | ATT-at | motive-to | there | to | go.V.ACT |
I went there to see the carvings in the south side of the mountain. |
kiň | te | i-ŋ | pî'dâ-daj | kâ-i | tru-zô. |
and | 3.INAN | at-inside | bee-COLL | ATT-at | find-V.ACT |
Because of the humming noise I looked inside the skull, and found a swarm of bees in it. |
Sometimes a serial postposition can be analyzed as involving an
omission of an obvious default noun between the first and second
postposition; for instance, the common sequence {dâm-ř kâ-i}
following an author's name:
taměs-ram | pejn-šam | dâm-ř | kâ-i | lju-zô | jǒj. |
Thomas-NAME | Payne-NAME.F | authorship-from | ATT-at | read-V.ACT | again |
I'm reading Thomas Payne [= some salient book he wrote] again. |
This could probably be interpreted as an abbreviated form of
or of
taměs-ram | pejn-šam | dâm-ř | twâ-cu | kâ-i | lju-zô | jǒj. |
Thomas-NAME | Payne-NAME.F | authorship-from | sentence-system | ATT-at | read-V.ACT | again |
I'm reading a book by Thomas Payne again. |
A time-postposition following another postposition is a common pattern,
especially with the state-transition postpositions {jâ-ř} and {jâ-o}, but
also with some others; e.g.,
mwĭl | jâ-ř | šin | šyj-ca | θǒ. |
sleep | state-from | immediately.after | clean-V.REFL | next |
After waking up I immediately bathed/showered. |
blâl | hǒl | jâ-o | ðij, | tyn | kǒ | ř | ruŋ-zô | mwe. |
frustration | total | state-to | shortly.before | place | DEM1 | from | go-V.ACT | IMP |
I should get out of here before I get totally frustrated. |
These before/after time postpositions can follow various object-case
postpositions, in which case a default verb appropriate to that case
is usually implied; for instance,
Purposive postpositions such as {rjâ-i} "in quest of" and {kujm-o}
"for the purpose of" can also commonly follow other postpositions,
including the state transition postpositions:
prym | jâ-o | rjâ-i | ŝrun | kâ-i | kâ-ku-zô. |
appreciation.of.beauty | state-to | quest-at | music | ATT-at | attention-hear-V.ACT |
I listen to music seeking to enter a state of appreciation-of-beauty. |
Indicating posture and position
A sequence of two spacetime postpositions — usually a part-whole
postposition followed by a contact postposition, both built on {i}
"at" — can be used to indicate posture and position. Usually a
locative verb of some kind is used with this sequence. For instance:
Ќ |
him |
sin |
su-ʝa-van |
1 |
back.part.of |
on |
standing-ROT-V.STATE |
I'm lying on my back. / lying supine. |
Ќ |
vim |
sin |
su-ʝa-van |
1 |
front.part.of |
on |
standing-ROT-V.STATE |
I'm lying on my front. / lying prone. |
Or if the second postposition in the sequence is a motion postposition
(built on {o} "to" or {ř} "from"), it can indicate getting into or out of
a given position.
ɱ |
ĵim |
son |
mlĭr-ʝa-ca |
zě'ĥâr. |
3 |
left.part.of |
onto |
rotate-ROT-V.REFL |
Zachary |
Zachary rolled over onto his right side. |
ɱ |
him |
sřn |
su |
jâ-o |
tîm'θĭ. |
3 |
back.part.of |
off.of |
standing |
state-to |
Timothy |
Timothy stands up from a supine position. |
More precise paths of motion
Two sequential motion postpositions, one built on {ř} "from" followed
by one built on {o} "to", can describe the path of motion relative to
the head of the postpositional phrase more precisely than a single
spacetime postposition can do. For instance,
rî'mâ |
θoŋ |
ruŋ-zô. |
house |
bottom.into |
go-V.ACT |
I go into the lower part of the house / into the basement. |
The {θoŋ} alone doesn't specify whether I'm starting out upstairs,
and going downstairs, or whether I'm starting out outdoors, and
entering the house on the basement level. Sometimes context
distinguishes, but one can also use two sequential postpositions:
rî'mâ |
sřŋ |
θoŋ |
ruŋ-zô. |
house |
top.out.of |
bottom.into |
go-V.ACT |
I go downstairs. |
rî'mâ |
źřj |
θoŋ |
ruŋ-zô. |
house |
west.from.near |
bottom.into |
go-V.ACT |
I enter the basement from the west side of the house. |
The {ź-} prefix isn't necessary here; replacing {źřj} with simple
{řj} would yield a less precise but still accurate meaning.
This is also how gjâ-zym-byn describes directions of motion more
generally, in situations where English, for instance, would use a
-ward adverb such as "north(ward)", "forward", etc.
purj |
břŋ |
ħoŋ |
ruŋ-źa-zô |
num. |
environment |
north.out.of |
south.into |
go-AUG-V.ACT |
wren |
The wren voyages south. |
ɱ |
ĵř |
co |
ƴâ-zô |
dě'pyw. |
3 |
right.from |
left.to |
walk-V.ACT |
crab |
The crab walks to its left. |
In the first example, the southward motion is described relative
to the environment as a whole; in the second, it's relative to the
crab's own body, with the
cataphoric pronoun {ɱ}
pointing forward to the crab, which hasn't been mentioned yet.
Similarly a combination of two before/after postpositions is used
to indicate direction of time-travel, e.g.,
sî'ðyr-źa-cu |
ðoj |
šř |
ruŋ-ƴu-zô. |
fight-AUG-system |
before.to.near |
after.from |
go-time-V.ACT |
I time-traveled back to shortly before the war. |
If the speaker just used {ðoj} by itself, the sentence would be
ambiguous as to whether they were going to that point in time from
some point even earlier before the war, or from a future point.
Abstract postpositions
The abstract case markers all derive from a root word followed by a
basic {i, o, ř} postposition.
tu-i | agent |
mĭ-i | topic |
ŋĭn-i | comment |
ʝâr-i | experiencer |
The relationships shown by the English possessive or the Greek genitive are
shown in various ways in gjâ-zym-byn:
ŝâj | having stuff |
ŝâj-i | of (belonging to) |
lĭw | personal relationship |
lĭw-i | of (related to) |
dâm | authorship |
dâm-ř | of (by) |
The partitive genitive would sometimes be translated with the suffix
{-na} ("made of" the substance described by the root). A few other concepts
denoted by prepositions in other languages are denoted by suffixes here as well:
{-ta} "without", {-ja} "according to, fitting".
Some other useful non-spacetime postpositions:
muw-i | subset of; one of; out of; among
|
syj | use, utility |
syj-i | with, using |
gân | cause, reason |
gân-ř | because of, on account of |
kujm | motive, goal, purpose, reason
|
kujm-o | in order to, for the purpose of
|
ðĭ | relationship
|
ðĭ-i | in some unspecified relationship with
|
{ðĭ-i} roughly corresponds to Esperanto's generic
preposition "je". Its most common use is to mark the object
of a stative verb when the subject must be marked with {mĭ-i}
rather than {ʝâr-i}, and no other postposition seems more
precisely fitting for the object. The possession and ownership verbs
{ŝâj-van} and {wuŋ-van} are the most common such verbs.
hajnlajn-šam | dâm-ř | twâ-cu |
Heinlein-NAME.F | authorship-from | sentence-system |
ðy-ðy-lwa-bô | ðĭ-i | wuŋ-van. |
five-five-approximate-ADJ | relationship-at | own-V.STATE |
I own about twenty-five books by Heinlein.
{muw-i} is used to indicate that the entity or group of entities
denoted by one noun phrase is a member or subset of another set. It
translates among other things some uses of the English phrases
"one of", "some of", "among" and
"out of":
When used to describe the proportion of a given set with a certain
property or engaging in a certain action, the noun used before {muw-i}
doesn't need to be repeated after it, just the numerator.
blâl-van | ŋî'bĭ | fĭw-bô | mĭ-i. |
annoyance-V.STATE | number | fiction-ADJ | TOP-at |
Four out of five doctors get annoyed at made-up statistics. |
There is an implied {fĭm-hôw-tla} between {muw-i} and
{ĉu-cŭ-bô} here, as in English "Four [doctors] out
of five doctors..."
Lexical dissimilation
When an abstract postposition would occur after the same noun that it's built upon, it is generally replaced with a plain spacetime postposition for the sake of euphony, or sometimes replaced with a less specific/accurate abstractpostposition. E.g., substituting a simplified postposition:
?mĭ | nǒ | i | gju-ŋĭn-zô | θě'mâ. |
topic | Q.WH | at | speak-explain-V.ACT | Thomas |
What topic did Thomas lecture about? |
— as opposed to {?mĭ nǒ mĭ-i}.
Substituting a less specific postposition:
— as opposed to {ŝĭw kǒ ŝĭw-i}. {ŝĭw-i} is normally used for the materials one uses up to do the action of the verb, while {syj-i} is used for tools that are relatively unaffected by a single use. But when the substanceused is so vaguely described as above, {syj-i} is used to avoid repeating {ŝĭw}so close together.
I call this phenomenon "lexical dissimilation." A similar kind of dissimilation occurs withthe suffix {-ma}
where it replaces a repeated instance of the same morpheme in a derived word. Some of the other postpositions this occurs most commonly with include {kujm-o} "for the purpose of," and {gân-ř} "because of."
Note on why I derive abstract postpositions from spacetime postpositions
A lot of natlangs and naturalistic conlangs cover a lot of their needs
for abstract relationship adpositions by metaphorically stretching the
basic meaning of a locational or temporal adposition. gzb does a lot
less of that (I won't say none at all); instead of, for instance,
using {si} to mean both "physically on top of" and "about the topic
of", or "hi" to mean both "physically behind" and "being the cause
of", it forms abstract relationship postpositions by compounding an
abstract noun root with one of the basic spatial relationship
postpositon. The spatio-temporal meaning of {i} remains intact when
it stands alone or combines with spacetime prefixes and suffixes; it
generally only gets stretched into abstract territory when it's
combined with an abstract noun, and then the noun it's combined with
generally indicates pretty clearly what that stretched meaning is.
Hypothetically, I could have simply extended the meanings of those
abstract nouns into postpositions by zero-derivation, the way English
does a lot of its derivation between the categories of noun, verb and
adjective. But the way gzb syntax works, that would have made a lot
of sentences hard to parse, I think. Or I could have lexicalized the
most common thematic-relationship concepts simply as postpositions,
and then used a nominalizer when I needed to use the corresponding
abstract noun; but then I would have either needed a more complex
morphophonology, to give room for more allowed wordforms in the
postposition class, or I still would have needed some way to derive
less-common thematic-relationship postpositions from abstract nouns.
Or I could have formed abstract postpositions from abstract nouns with
one or more normal suffixes, the way I produce conjunctions from noun
stems with {-šar}, adjectives with {-bô} and others, etc. But it
seemed more intuitive to use the core spacetime postpositions, which
were already there, instead of adding more morphemes and making words
that were already longer than most natlang equivalents even longer
(the shortest possible suffix of normal form is two phonemes, CV).
Also, I would have needed three postposition-making suffixes to
produce a system as flexible and expressive as the one gzb has now —
notice the way the variation between the spacetime postpositions {i,
o, ř} marks aspect in {jâ-i}, {jâ-o}, and {jâ-ř}; there are other
triplets of related case postpositions as well.
I intended that the postposition system should have its inevitable
complexity all on the surface, manifesting as a large number of
postpositions with fairly specific meanings and uses, instead of it
being hidden, as in most natlangs, which have a smaller number of
adpositions and cases but each has a bewildering arrray of meanings
and rules for when to use it. However, I'm not sure I succeeded; in
the process of pulling most (not all) of the unavoidable complexity to
the surface, I may have created some otherwise avoidable complexity.
Modifiers (adjectives/adverbs)
Modifiers are formed from root substantives by addition of appropriate
suffixes. gjâ-zym-byn does not make a morphological distinction
between adjectives and adverbs, though in practice some modifiers
always modify verbs or modifiers. Modifiers, whether non-derived
particles or derived words, always follow their heads, with a handful
of exceptions: interjections like {hwǒ}, evidentiality adverbs derived with
{-pôm}, and ordinal
adverbs derived with {-saw}, which can optionally come at the
beginning of a clause instead of after the main verb (or the most verb-like
postpositional phrase, in a sentence with no verb).
If the root noun denotes a quality or state, use {-bô} to form the
adjective meaning "having this quality, being in this state". Other
suffixes can also be used with this kind of root.
bâm | newness |
bâm-bô | new |
bâm-za | of newness |
bâm-tan | like new |
bâm-cô | age, non-newness |
bâm-cô-bô | old |
If the root noun denotes a concrete entity or type of entity, {-bô}
and {-cô} wouldn't be appropriate, but other modifier derivations
are possible:
râm | cat |
râm-za | pertaining to cats |
râm-tan | resembling a cat |
pwĭm | water |
pwĭm-za | of water |
pwĭm-tan | like water |
but:
râm-rô | independent |
pwĭm-rô | humble |
{-rô} specifies an idiomatically selected quality of the root
substantive. It's similar to "-um" in Esperanto — not
all concrete roots have a defined {rô}-adjective.
If the root denotes an action or relation, certain other suffixes are
appropriate.
lju | reading |
lju-fwa | causing to read |
lju-faj | readable, legible |
lju-gô | worth reading |
If the root denotes a mindstate, an adjective formed with {-fwa}
describes the circumstances or qualities that conduce to it, and an
adjective formed with {-bô} describes the person who experiences it.
In gjâ-zym-byn most or all subjective qualities are named by a root
mindstate-word plus {-fwa}.
ħum | fear |
ħum-bô | afraid |
ħum-fwa | terrifying |
prym | appreciation of beauty |
prym-fwa | beautiful |
prym-bô | in awe of something beautiful |
prym-cô | distaste for ugliness |
prym-cô-fwa | ugly |
For more details on derivation of modifiers, see
the corresponding sections of the
derivational morphology document.
Attribution
Modifiers as attributes normally follow the nouns they modify.
gâm ny-bô | small picture
|
gâm pân-kwa | multicolored picture
|
gâm prym-fwa | beautiful picture
|
However, with the conjunction {če}, a modifier can be bound
to its head in reverse order:
The same rule goes for modifiers of verbs:
ruŋ-zô ĵwy-bô | to move quickly
|
ruŋ-zô žu-bô | to move carefully
|
With the reversal conjunction,
ĵwy-bô če ruŋ-zô | = ruŋ-zô ĵwy-bô
|
Usually {če} is used as an
error correction
mechanism.
Here is a sentence with a modified noun as patient and a modified
verb:
gâm |
ny-bô |
ĥy-i |
tâň-zô |
ƴum-bô |
mâ-ħa |
kwǒ. |
picture |
small |
PAT-at |
take-V.ACT |
fraud-ADJ |
person-ATD4 |
some |
Some scoundrel has taken the small picture in a fraudulent way. |
Predication
Modifiers as predicates normally occur with the postpositions
{ŋĭn-i} (comment) or {jâ-i} (state), with the noun phrase whose
referent they're predicated of marked by {mĭ-i} or (more typically)
left unmarked in final subject position.
Note, however, that in predicate position a root or stem
signifying a state or quality doesn't have to take the {-bô} suffix,
as it does in attributive position.
ny |
ŋĭn-i |
gâm |
pǒ. |
smallness |
CMT-at |
picture |
DEM3 |
That picture is small. |
(There are archaic sentence in many texts, and in these grammar
documents, where a {-bô} adjective is used in predicate position.
Updating the grammar document and all its sample sentences to
reflect the current state of the language is an ongoing task.)
prym-fwa |
ŋĭn-i |
gâm |
ny-bô. |
aesthetic.pleasure-CAUS |
CMT-at |
picture |
small-ADJ |
The small picture is beautiful. |
The state transition postpositions {jâ-o} (becoming) and {jâ-ř}
(ceasing to be) are used similarly.
hwǒ, |
pî'râ |
jâ-o |
gâm. |
INTJ |
flame |
state-to |
picture |
Oh no, the picture's caught on fire! |
prym-fwa |
jâ-ř |
gâm |
ĥy-pî'râ-bô. |
aesthetic.pleasure-CAUS |
state-from |
picture |
PAT-flame-ADJ |
The burned picture ceases to be beautiful. |
{ŋĭn-i} and {jâ-i} are interchangeable in many contexts; one rule
of thumb is that {ŋĭn-i} is used with more subjective qualities,
while {jâ-i} with more objective ones.
{jâ-i} also tends to be used for more transitory qualities, and
{ŋĭn-i} with more durable ones. Also, if the subject is an animate
entity marked by {ʝâr-i} "experiencer", then the predicate modifier
is marked by {jâ-i} or the state transition variants thereof, not
{ŋĭn-i}.
mwĭl |
jâ-ř |
gî'lu |
ʝâr-i. |
sleep |
state-from |
wolverine |
experiencer-at |
The wolverine is waking up. |
ĥul |
jâ-o |
gî'lu |
ʝâr-i. |
anger |
state-to |
wolverine |
experiencer-at |
The wolverine is getting angry. |
Comparison
Comparative forms of modifiers and (especially stative) verbs are
formed with the suffixes {-sra} "more" and {-ĵar}
"less". Applied to modifiers formed from quality-stems,
they come before the {-bô} adjectivizing suffix:
hum | depth |
hum-bô | deep |
hum-sra | quality of being deeper than something else |
hum-sra-bô | deeper |
hum-ĵar-bô | less deep |
hum-cô | height |
hum-cô-bô | high, tall |
hum-cô-sra | quality of being higher/taller than something else |
hum-cô-sra-bô | higher, taller |
hum-cô-ĵar-bô | less high, shorter |
Applied to other modifiers, formed with suffixes other than {-bô},
the {-sra} and {-ĵar} normally come at the end of the word. For
instance, with the causative suffix {-fwa}:
prym | appreciation of beauty |
prym-fwa | beautiful |
prym-fwa-sra | more beautiful |
but this is also possible, though rarer:
prym-sra | more intensely/vividly appreciating something's beauty |
prym-sra-fwa | causing someone to more vividly appreciate something's beauty |
With most other modifier suffixes this inversion wouldn't make sense,
though:
râm-tan-sra | more catlike |
žâj-dô-ĵar | less sinful |
pwĭm-da-sra | wetter |
ðâ-ja-ĵar | less logical |
lju-gô-sra | more worth reading |
When turning one of these comparative modifiers into a stative verb,
replace {-bô} with {-van} or add {-van} after the comparative suffix:
hum-sra-van | to be deeper |
prym-fwa-sra-van | to be more beautiful |
gjâ-zym-byn doesn't have a morphologically distinct comparative
and superlative. If a comparative modifier or verb form occurs with
an explicit standard of comparison, it would generally be translated
into English as "more/less X" or "X-er"; if it
occurs with no standard of comparison, the implicit comparison may be,
depending on context, to the same entity in the past, or to some other
recently mentioned entity, or to all other things of the kind, or all
things of that kind that are in context at the moment. In the latter
cases it would be translated as "most/least X" or
"X-est". Inexplicit comparison can be in attribute form (a
modifier applying to a head noun or verb within a single noun phrase
or verb phrase) or predicate form (the modifier being in a separate
comment or state postpositional phrase, applying to a head noun that's
in a topic or experiencer postpositional phrase):
mwĭl-ŝra-van | tâ | ĵĭn-sra | ʝâr-i. |
sleep-tending-V.STATE | sibling | young-COMP | experiencer-at |
The youngest of the siblings is sleepy. |
sâr-ĵar | ŋĭn-i | pě'pâ-daj | pǒ. |
order-COMP.NEG | CMT-at | paper-mass | that |
That mass of papers is less orderly [than other such] / is the least orderly. |
huw-sra | jâ-i | mâ-bâm | kǒ. |
happy-COMP | state-at | person-new | this |
This baby is happier [than other [recently mentioned?] babies] / is the happiest. |
Or a comparative modifier can be an attribute of a verb, i.e. an
adverb:
gzb has two ways of relating the head noun of the comparative modifier
or the subject of the comparative verb to the standard it's being
compared with. One is with the comparative conjunction {θe}
(as, than); this
is archaic, the
comparative conjunction usually being used now only for
equality-comparison. In this form the subject of the comparison is
linked within its topic postpositional phrase with the standard of
comparison:
*hwǒ, | lâŋ-sra-van | twâ-cu-θuň | kǒ | θe | {gormenħast-wam} | mĭ-i. |
INT | long-COMP-V.STATE | sentence-system-story | this | than | Gormenghast-NAME.P | TOP-at |
Whoa, this novel is longer than Gormenghast! |
Nowadays (since 2008), the standard of comparison is put in a separate
postpositional phrase marked with {dî'fu-i}, "compared
with". ({dî'fu} derives from the name of the Unix command
"diff".)
The comparative suffixes can also occur with such quantifier clitics
as {reŋ} (many) and modifier clitics as {mje} (in the past):
ƥ | dâm-ř | twâ-cu | reŋ-sra | ðĭ-i | wuŋ-Ќ-van. |
3 | authorship-from | sentence-system | many-COMP | relation-at | own-1-V.STATE |
I own more books by her [than someone else does / than by some other author / than I used to]. |
ƥ | ĥun-i | gju-môj | mje-ĵar. |
3 | meet-at | speak-V.RECP | past-COMP.NEG |
I have spoken with him more recently [than someone else has / than with other people recently mentioned]. |
There are a couple of kinds of qualified comparatives; they occur only
as predicates, not as attributes. Subset comparison corresponds to
English "one of the most/least...", and uses the subset postposition
{muw-i}:
Ranked comparison corresponds to English "the second-most X", "the
third-least Y", etc. It is unique in not actually using the suffixes
{-sra} and {-ĵar} at all, but an ordinal number in a comment phrase
with the compared quality in a generic relational phrase:
Φu | ðĭ-i | ĉu-pa | ŋĭn-i | tam-ram | mĭ-i. |
mass | relationship-at | two-ORD | CMT-at | Tom-NAME | TOP-at |
Tom is the second most massive. (= With respect to mass, Tom is second.) |
ŋy | ðĭ-i | se-ĉu-pa | ŋĭn-i | kentâwrus-wam | mĭ-i. |
distance | relationship-at | minus-two-ORD | CMT-at | Centaurus-NAME.P | TOP-at |
Centaurus is second least distant / second nearest. [In a discussion of stars; Sol of course is the nearest.] |
Subset and ranked comparison can be combined to give an explicit
standard of comparison, e.g.:
Explicit comparisons can be made with {dî'fu-i} in relation to
a quantified standard, e.g.:
mâ | jǒm | dî'fu-i | ĵlân-sra-van | hektor-ram. |
person | most.of | compare-at | wise-COMP-V.STATE | Hector-NAME |
Hector is wiser than most people / than the majority of people. |
Explicit superlatives generally use {dî'fu-i}, with a standard
quantified with {ble}, "the rest of":
mruň | ble | dî'fu-i | hum-cô-sra-van | evřest-wam. |
mountain | rest.of | compare-at | deep-OPP2-COMP-V.STATE | Everest-NAME.P |
Everest is taller than the rest of the mountains / is the tallest mountain. |
And a locative or temporal phrase or {muw-i} subset phrase can give a more
explicit context within which the superlative is valid:
The comparative suffixes generally don't apply to entity stems.
However, they are occasionally used with number or unit-of-measure
stems, meaning "more than" or "less than" the amount specified by the
stem:
tĭm-sra | a set with more than a hundred members |
tĭm-sra-bô | more than a hundred |
gĭ-sra-gla i | later than eleven o'clock |
kî'grâ-ĵar | less than a kilogram |
With verbs (whether active or stative) derived from process stems, the
comparative suffixes mean "to do said action/undergo said process
more/less intensely":
grĭ-ĵar-van | šĭm-ĉa | kǒ. |
function-COMP.NEG-V.STATE | algorithm-tool | this |
This computer functions less well [than other computers / than it used to]. |
These comparative action/process verbs can have explicit standards
of comparison with {dî'fu-i} or {muw-i} as well.
Some ambiguous comparisons can be disambiguated by using serial postpositions,
putting a case postposition before {dî'fu-i} to indicate the role the standard
of comparison plays in the sentence (besides being the standard of comparison):
Does she love Mary more than she loves Sarah, or does she love Mary more than
Sarah does? Adding {ʝâr-i} or {mĭ-i} between {sě'râ} and {dî'fu-i}
disambiguates whether we're comparing two experiencers or two topics:
An example with an active verb:
Equality comparison uses the comparative conjunction {θe} and some
sameness or similarity adverb on the stative verb, adjective, or
state-postposition:
twâ-cu | kǒ | θe | twâ-cu | tǒ | mĭ-i | lâŋ-van | sâm-bô. |
sentence-system | DEM1 | as | sentence-system | DEM2 | TOP-at | long-V.STATE | same-ADJ |
This book is just as long as that book. |
For more examples of equality comparison, see the section of the
derivational morphology document on
mathematical equation sentences.
One can also make an explicit comparison with an entity as it was in
the past:
pym-fwa-van | sâm-bô | tam-ram | θe | ƥ | mje. |
amusement-CAUS-V.STATE | same-ADJ | Tom-NAME | as | 3 | past |
Tom is as funny as his past self / is as funny as ever. |
ɱ | mje | dî'fu-i | žu-sô-sra | jâ-i | vĭrě-ram. |
3 | past | compare-at | care-tending-COMP | state-at | Vera-NAME |
Vera is more careful than her past self / than she used to be. |
Or one could use {ler} "future" or a postpositional phrase specifying
a particular time period to be more specific about what other point
on an entity's world-line you're comparing its present self to.
Pronouns
The simple personal pronouns are:
Ќ | I, me |
ť | you |
Ł | "one", "they" (generic) "someone" (unspecified) |
ƥ, ɱ | he, she, they (refers to spirits, humans, animals) |
te, ŋe | it, they (plants, inanimate objects, abstractions) |
{ƥ} points backward to a previously mentioned person or group,
{ɱ} forward to somone(s) not already mentioned by name.
Similarly {te} and {ŋe} point backward and forward to their
referents. Technically, {ƥ} and {te} are anaphoric pronouns,
while {ɱ} and {ŋe} are cataphoric pronouns.
Note there is no plural first person pronoun. "We" could be expressed by:
Ќ-ť | inclusive we: I and you |
Ќ-ƥ / Ќ-ɱ | exclusive we: I and someone else, I and some others |
Ќ-ť-ƥ / Ќ-ť-ɱ | very inclusive we: I and you and other(s) |
The other pronouns can be marked plural by appending a quantifier
clitic, or a number adjective, but in some contexts this is not
necessary; ť, ƥ, ɱ can refer to plural antecedents even
without such explicit pluralization. Ќ would not be pluralized
with a number or quantifier, only in one of the ways mentioned above
(unless the speaker were a group mind or hive, perhaps). ("Royal
We" would be translated by appending a respectful affix to the
first-person pronoun.)
I have not been perfectly consistent about using {ƥ, ɱ} vs.
{te, ŋe} for body parts. I am leaning toward consistently using
the animate pronouns, but for now the animate and inanimate pronouns
are interchangeable for reference to body parts.
{Ł} is glossed above as "one," but it has a broader meaning than "one" in
formal English or "on" in French. It can refer to a generic, nonspecific
person, in a statement that applies to people in general or a broad class of
people, but it can also refer to a specific person whose identity is unknown or
irrelevant to the current discussion, similar to the way the passive voice in
many Indo-European languages deemphasizes or leaves out the agent. In this
sense it could be glossed as "someone."
ƴâw |
ŝâj-o |
čĭm |
ĥy-i |
vâ-oŋ-fwa-Ł-zô |
źǒ. |
dog |
posses-to |
chocolate |
PAT-at |
digestion-into-CAUS-3.GEN-V.ACT |
IMP.NEG |
One mustn't give chocolate to dogs. |
Ќ |
sim |
ʝym-daj |
ĥy-i |
ĥâ-Ł-zô. |
1 |
top.part.of |
hair/leaf-mass |
PAT-at |
cut-3.GEN-V.ACT |
I got my hair cut / Someone cut my hair. |
gzb has no interrogative or relative pronouns per se, but uses the
interrogative particle {nǒ}, or the relativizer {lǒ}, with various
nouns or noun phrases to serve those purposes.
mâ lǒ | who, whom |
mâ nǒ | who? whom? |
gâ lǒ | which |
gâ nǒ | what? which? |
Almost any noun phrase can be interrogated or relativized, but in practice a few generic
nouns tend to get used frequently with these particles. See under
Information questionse and
Relative clauses for details.
The non-personal pronouns are:
ce | this, that; stands for a whole situation described previously |
že | this, that; stands for a fact or situation about to be described |
re | there, thither, thence; stands for a recently mentioned place |
{že} typically points forward to the content of a
{hǒŋ} subordinate clause.
že | mĭ-i | gju-zô | ƥ | tu-i, | hǒŋ | gjâ | kǒ | mĭ-i |
this | TOP-at | speak-V.ACT | 3 | AGT-at | that | language | DEM1 | TOP-at |
syj-faj | heŋ | źe | ŋĭn-i. |
use-able | not | very | CMT-at. |
She talked about how useless my language was.
ce | mĭ-i | sjum-van, | wǒj | Ќ | ĥy-i | hyw-fwa-zô | ce | gân-ř |
this | TOP-at | thankful-V.STATE | because | 1 | PAT-at | know-CAUS-V.ACT | this | cause-from |
luŋ | mĭ-i. |
detachment | TOP-at |
I was thankful for that, because it taught me something about detachment.
{re} is a place pronoun, similar to "y" in French. It is normally followed
by a spacetime postposition to express its role in the sentence, just like
a noun phrase referring to a place.
re |
i |
tyn-van |
bĭr-ĵwa |
reŋ. |
there |
at |
place-V.STATE |
beer-place |
many |
Peter and Zachary traveled to Athens, Georgia. There are a lot of bars there. |
However, {re} can be the object of an abstract case postposition as well.
tâŋ |
reŋ |
ði |
sânfrânsĭsko-wam-la |
o |
ruŋ-źa-zô. |
personal.era |
many |
ago |
San.Francisco-NAME.P-ATD1 |
to |
go-AUG-V.ACT |
re |
kâ-i |
prym-źa-van, |
mǒn |
pwĭm-by-da |
ŋĭn-i |
re. |
there |
ATT-at |
aesthetic.appreciation-AUG-V.STATE |
although |
water-air-full |
CMT-at |
there |
Many years ago I traveled to San Francisco. I found it very beautiful, although it was foggy. |
In the second clause, {re} is the object of attention, and in the third, {re} is
the topic (the topic postposition being left out in sentence-final position).
One can add modifiers to pronouns, indicating number,
gender, age, etc., if necessary to clarify which of several
previously-mentioned entities is intended.
!ť | pen | tu-i | ruŋ-zô | mwe | mruň | on. |
you | all | AGT-at | go-V.ACT | IMP | mountain | to |
Y'all go to the mountain.
vlym | bâm-bô | ĥy-i | kâj-zô | ƥ-ŝy | srǒ | tu-o | ƥ-mym | ŝâj-o. |
clothing | new-ADJ | PAT-at | exchange-V.ACT | 3-female | several | AGT-to | 3-self | possession-to. |
They buy new clothes.
Conjunctions
{gjâ-zym-byn} has several kinds of conjunctions.
One shows the relative truth or falsity of two independent clauses.
Words of this type are derived by compounding phonemes from a truth
table:
first clause | second clause | logic function |
T | T | ŝ = TT, k = TF, p = FT, f = FF |
T | F |
F | T | w = T, (null) = F |
F | F | oň = T, iň = F |
So for instance:
kiň | and - TFFF |
ŝwiň | or (inclusive) - TTTF |
pwiň | or (exclusive), unless - FTTF |
foň | neither/nor - FFFT |
koň | equivalence; if and only if — both are true or both false. TFFT |
(In practice, these are rarely used, except for {kiň}, and I don't think I've
ever used any of the 11 other conjunctions one could theoretically form from
this table.)
Another kind links two clauses and shows their causal relation (or
surprising lack thereof); like "because, therefore, however, but" in
English. They're also formed with a matrix.
Prefix elements:
ŝ- | logical cause | (therefore, because) |
w- | effective cause | (therefore, because) |
ʝ- | evidence, inference | (therefore, because) |
m- | not hindered | (however, even though, in spite of, but) |
Suffix elements:
-ǒn | 1st clause, therefore (however) 2nd clause |
-ǒj | 1st clause, because (even though) 2nd clause |
ĉu | pe | ðy | θe | fy | mĭ-i | sâm-van, | ŝǒn |
2 | plus | 5 | = | 7 | TOP-at | same-V.STATE | therefore.logically |
fy | se | ðy | θe | ĉu | mĭ-i | sâm-van. |
7 | minus | 5 | = | 2 | TOP-at | same-V.STATE. |
2 + 5 = 7, therefore 7 minus 5 = 2.
lju-sô | ŋĭn-i, | wǒn | kâj-zô | twâ-cu-vuj | reŋ | ĥy-i. |
read-tending.to | CMT-at, | therefore.in.fact | transact-V.ACT | sentence-system-concrete | many | PAT-at |
I am readful, so I buy many books.
{ʝǒj} and {ʝǒn} show a relationship between two clauses where the truth of
one clause is inferred or deduced from the more obviously evident truth of the other.
ƥ | wuŋ-i | ƴâ-ĉa | kâ-i | rĭm-Ќ-van | heŋ. |
3 | owning-at | go-tool | ATT-at | see-1-V.STATE | not |
He must have already gone, because I don't see his car. |
zym-zô, | ʝǒn | bĭŋ-van. |
think-V.ACT | therefore.inference | exist-V.STATE |
I think, therefore (I deduce that) I am.
The {-ǒj} forms reverse the causal order:
râm | mĭ-i | pwĭ-cô-van, | wǒj | ƥ | mĭ-i | pwĭm-da | ŋĭn-i. |
cat | TOP-at | delight-OPP2-V.STATE, | because.fact | 3 | TOP-at | water-full | CMT-at |
The cat is miserable because it's wet.
{mǒn, mǒj} correspond to "although" and
"but"; there is a mirative element in one of the two clauses
joined by these, the truth of one clause being unexpected or
surprising in light of the truth of the other clause:
helenike-lam | mĭ-i | suŋ-van | heŋ, | mǒn | kun-hôw-ca | mje | te | kâ-i. |
Greek-NAME.L | TOP-at | know.how-V.STATE | not, | although | know-CAUS2-V.REFL | PAST | 3 | ATT-at |
I'm not fluent in Greek though I studied it awhile ago.
vlym-srĭw | ĥy-i | šyj-zô | žu-bô, | mǒj | te | im | ver | ħĭwm | gǒ. |
clothing-legs | PAT-at | clean-V.ACT | care-ADJ | but | 3.INAN | part.of | still | stain | behold |
I washed the pants carefully, but look, the stain is still there. |
The conjunctions {mǒn} and {mǒj} are sometimes used in combination:
mǒj | Ќ | ŝâj-i | ƴâ-ĉa | mĭ-i | grĭ-cô-van. |
but | 1 | possession-at | move-tool | TOP-at | function-OPP2-V.STATE |
Although I would like to come to your house, my car is broken. |
A third type of conjunction primarily works with numbers to show
arithmetic operations. Some of them are used by analogy with other
words and phrases.
pe | plus; additive 'and' |
ke | multiplied by; synergetic 'and' |
se | minus; 'except' |
ðe | divided by, per; contrasted with |
me | raised to the power of |
zej | range operator: "X zej Y", the set of numbers from X to Y inclusive |
Examples of the math/phrasal conjunctions:
ɱ-ŝy | pe | Ќ | tu-i | vâ-oŋ-zô. |
3-female | and | 1 |
AGT-at | digestion-into-V.ACT |
She and I ate together. |
{pe} can be used to link verbs within a single clause which have the
same subject and object.
Ќ | lĭw-i | tâ-ma-bâm | mĭ-i | fâ-van | pe | pym-van. |
1 | relationship-at | sibling-meta-new | TOP-at | love-V.STATE | and | amusement-V.STATE |
I love my baby cousin and find [her] amusing. |
In these cases the two verbs not only have the same subject but have the
same kind of relationship to their objects: patient in the first case,
focus or topic in the second. But between verbs with different
subjects, or with the same subject but different relationships to
their object, the clausal conjunction {kiň} must be used to translate
"and".
In English one might say "I am writing and revising a new story", "a
new story" being the direct object of both verbs; but gzb has no
"direct objects" as such, just patients, objects-of-result,
objects-of-attention, and so forth. Here each verb has a different
relationship to its object and requires a different postposition to
mark it, so the pronoun {te} is used resumptively and {ĥy-i} shows
that {byn-zô} is modifying a now existing story, not creating a new
one like {lju-θaj-zô}.
Besides its core meaning of "multiplied by", {ke} is used
between non-mathematical nouns to mean "and", but implying a
closer, synergistic connection between the nouns or noun phrases
linked by it than {pe}.
ɱ-ĉu | mĭ-i | rě'ĵy | ke | rě'ĵy-θaj | jâ-i. |
3-two | TOP-at | wife | and | wife-OPP1 | state-at |
They are wife and husband.
{ke} can also be used to link proper names of husband and wife,
co-authors, or collaborators; in this case the names are compounded
into one word with -ke- as a kind of hyphen, and the name suffix
{-ram} or {-šam} is usually only used once, not after each name.
Φě'ĥu-cu | mĭ-i | krĭ-gâm-zô | tam-ke-ser'ě-ram. |
elephant-system | TOP-at | create-picture-V.ACT | Tom-and-Sarah-NAME |
Tom and Sarah painted a picture of a herd of elephants. |
{se} "minus" is
incorporated into number-words to form negative numbers,
and
used phrasally to express subtraction.
With non-numbers, it means "except", "with the exception of":
bâm-sra |
kâ-i |
lju-zô |
gwe. |
new-COMP |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
already |
I've read all of David Mitchell's novels except his newest. |
{ðe} "divided by" is used with number-words to derive
words/phrases for fractions.
With other kinds of words, it means "contrasted with":
bě'lâm |
ðe |
mwĭň |
mĭ-i |
dlâw-Ł-van |
dî'fu-zô. |
embarrassment.privacy.violation |
CONTRAST |
embarrassment.subject |
TOP-at |
duty-3.GEN-V.STATE |
distinguish-V.ACT |
One must distinguish embarrassment at violated privacy from embarrassment at talking about emotionally sensitive subjects. |
{me} "raised to the power of" is incorporated into
kinship terms to indicate more distant relationships.
It's also used with units of measure and dimensional terms to indicate higher
dimensions, e.g. to derive volume from a basic word for distance.
kyn | parent
|
kyn-ma | grandparent
|
kyn-me-dâ | great-grandparent
|
ŋy | distance
|
ŋy-ma | area
|
ŋy-me-dâ | volume
|
mě'tyr | meter
|
mě'tyr-ma | square meter
|
mě'tyr-me-dâ | cubic meter
|
{se} and {ðe} are most commonly used in the corpus with numbers ({se}
is most commonly used in the phrase {gě'dĭm se-cĭ-pa}, day
minus-one'th = yesterday), while {pe} and {ke} are far more commonly
used with ordinary stems. This is probably because simply concatenating
numbers in compounds
multiplies or adds them,
according to their increasing or decreasing magnitude.
The range conjunction {zej} has primarily been used with number
words, signifying a range of numbers from one to another inclusive:
ðy zej fy | five to seven, i.e. the integers 5, 6 and 7 or the real numbers from 5.0 to 7.0 depending on context
|
ðy-pa zej fy-pa | the fifth through seventh items in a sequence
|
ðy-gla zej fy-gla | from five a.m. to seven a.m. in the morning
|
E.g.,
tyn-van |
heŋ |
ŋî'bĭ |
gĭ-rô. |
be.located-V.STATE |
not |
number |
eleven-QUAL |
There are no prime numbers from 114 to 126. |
θuň-lâŋ-kwĭ |
pǒ |
im |
ĉu-pa |
zej |
ðy-pa |
kâ-i |
lju-zô. |
story-long-series |
DEM3 |
part.of |
two-ORD |
RANGE |
five-ORD |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
I've read the second through fifth novels in that series. |
hâ-gla |
zej |
ħy-gla |
i |
gjâ |
kǒ |
ĥy-i |
byn-zô. |
seventeen-ORD.T |
RANGE |
nineteen-ORD.T |
at |
language |
DEM1 |
PAT-at |
tinker-V.ACT |
I tinkered with this language from 5pm to 7pm. |
It can also signify an approximate, uncertain number that's somewhere
in a certain range, depending on context.
I've recently (May 2010) discovered that {zej} can be used with any
pair of modifiers in the same semantic domain, not just with numbers.
It can express a transition from one state or quality to another
within a common continuum, or that various members of the set denoted
by the head noun have values for the meta-quality in a given range;
for instance:
A fourth kind of conjunction are individual words to fill miscellaneous needs.
hǒŋ | introduces object subordinate
clauses: 'I think that...', 'He asked whether...' |
ðǒŋ | introduces subject
subordinate clauses: 'It's obvious that...', 'It's uncertain
whether...'
|
Φǒ | introduces quotations: 'He said "..."' |
θe | comparative (as, than) |
šej | or; also known as; that is to say |
rej | exclusive or (phrasal, contrast with clausal {pwiň}) |
hej | inclusive or (phrasal, contrast with clausal {ŝwiň}) |
ĉǒ | if (conditions beyond speaker's control) |
bǒ | if (conditions within speaker's control) |
dlen | else, otherwise |
ĉe | concatenation conjunction |
Examples of some of the subordinate clause conjunctions:
twâ-zô | hǒŋ | ruŋ-zô | ƥ. |
say-V.ACT | that | come-V.ACT | 3 |
I said that he's coming.
twâ-zô | Φǒ | {ruŋ-zô | ƥ.} |
say-V.ACT | QUOTE | come-V.ACT | 3 |
I said "He's coming."
See also the main section on subordinate
clauses.
The conjunction {šej} can sometimes be glossed as "also known as"; it
links two different names or descriptions of the same entity, or a
main title and a subtitle.
dâm-ř | θuň | kâ-i. |
authorship-from | story | ATT-at |
Have you read any stories by Arouet, also known as Voltaire? |
ruŋ-zô | vjurm-zô | sĭr'ĭl-ram. |
go-V.ACT | visit-V.ACT | Cyril-NAME |
Cyril went to visit his oldest friend Cecil. |
It can also link two clauses that describe the same event or situation in different
ways.
vlym | oŋ | tyn-ca | šej | Ќ | ĉon | vlym | ĥy-i | tyn-zô. |
clothing | into | place-V.REFL | AKA | 1 | around | clothing | PAT-at | place-V.ACT |
I put myself into clothes, or surround myself with clothes. [= I get dressed.] |
{ĉǒ}, {bǒ} and {dlen} are discussed under
Conditional clauses.
{ĉe} is the concatenation conjunction, used between number words to
build up identifier strings (e.g., phone numbers or other arbitrary
identifiers with little or no intended mathematical signififance).
Various additional conjunctions can be derived from various stems with
the suffix {-šar}:
huw-fwa-ƥ-ca | ru-šar | ŝrun | kâ-i | ku-van. |
happy-CAUS-3-V.REFL | way-CONJ | music | ATT-at | hear-V.STATE |
She cheers herself up by listening to music. |
suŋ-van | pǒl-ram | ru-šar | pjân-zô | prym-fwa. |
know.how-V.STATE | Paul-NAME | way-CONJ | piano-V.ACT | aesthetic.appreciation-CAUS |
Paul is skilled at playing the piano beautifully. |
gju-ƥ-zô | mĭ-šar | mâl-cô-van | ķe | ħĭ | mĭ-i. |
talk-3-V.ACT | topic-CONJ | precise-OPP2-V.STATE | too.much | experiment | TOP-at |
He talked about how the experiment was too imprecise. |
gâm-daj | kâ-i | rĭm-van. |
picture-collection | ATT-at | see-V.STATE |
I went to the art museum to see
the Ensor
exhibit. |
Note that when a {kujm-šar} clause is negated, it is with the {źǒ} imperative
negative (see under
"Epistemic particles"):
ŋwĭm-ť-zô |
mwe |
kujm-šar |
ħâň-ƥ-zô |
ver |
źǒ. |
replace-2-V.ACT |
IMP |
purpose-CONJ |
yell-3-V.ACT |
still |
IMP.NEG |
Change the baby's diaper so they will stop yelling. |
Note the distinction in syntax between postpositions and
conjunctions formed from the same root:
ĥun-pĭw | o | ruŋ-zô. |
meeting-game | to | go-V.ACT |
I stayed home and read instead of going to the gaming party. |
See also the discussion of
{-šar} in the derivational morphology document.
Correlatives, demonstratives, interrogative and relative pronouns
Question-words and relative pronouns are formed by attaching a clitic
to any root word. For instance,
nǒ | what?, which? |
mâ-nǒ | who? [which person?] |
nu-nǒ | when? [at which moment?] |
vĭj-nǒ | when? [during what period?] |
ru-nǒ | how? [in what manner?] |
{nǒ} can be embedded into a verb.
?gâ-nǒ-ta-van.
thing-Q.WH-without-V.STATE
What am I missing?
?ryň-nǒ-zô | ť | tu-i. |
action-Q.WH-V.ACT | 2 | AGT-at |
What are you doing?
Relative pronouns are formed in a similar way with the clitic {lǒ}.
(Note that gjâ-zym-byn uses distinct relative and interrogative pronouns.
English, French, and Esperanto all make do with a single series of
pronouns for both relatives and interrogatives in wh-, qu-, and ki-
respectively.)
lǒ | which, that |
mâ-lǒ | who, that |
tyn-lǒ | where |
The demonstrative clitics (this, that...) are similar in form.
kǒ | this near me |
tǒ | that near you |
pǒ | that far from us |
Terms like "here", "there", "now", "then", and so forth are formed
by applying these clitics to various root words; usually a
postposition is required as well.
tyn kǒ i | here (at this place) |
šun pǒ i | there (in that region) |
tyn tǒ o | thither (toward you) |
The distinction between {kǒ} and {tǒ} can be interesting with
respect to time:
nu kǒ i | now (at this moment when I am speaking/writing this) |
vĭj tǒ i | now (during the period when you are hearing/reading this) |
vĭj | tǒ | i | grâm | kǒ | ĥy-i | pî'râ-zô | mwe. |
time | DEM2 | during | message | DEM1 | PAT-at | fire-V.ACT | IMP |
Burn this message now [when you read it].
Terms like "anyone, everyone, no one", etc., are formed by following
ordinary root words with various quantifier particles or compounding with
number root words.
mâ pen | everyone |
mâ-bâ | no one |
mâ kwǒ | somebody, anyone |
tyn pen | everywhere |
vĭj-bâ | never |
Definiteness
gzb does not mark definiteness pervasively with articles or
inflections as do English, French, Basque, and some other languages.
Nouns are by default unmarked for definiteness, though context will
usually clarify whether any instance of a class or a particular member
is meant. When context is insufficient, gzb has several particles
that can be used to mark definiteness or indefiniteness, including
the demonstrative particles and some of the quantifiers.
rî'mâ | rjâ-i. |
house | quest-at |
I'm looking for a/some/the house. [ambiguous in itself, context may clarify] |
Any house will do, perhaps; or the speaker is vague about what house
or kind of house he's looking for.
rî'mâ | sǒ | rjâ-i. |
house | certain | quest-at |
I'm looking for a certain house. |
Here the speaker knows the specific house he's looking for but isn't sure if the listener does.
rî'mâ | pǒ | rjâ-i. |
house | that.3 | quest-at |
I'm looking for the/that house. |
Here the speaker and listener have already agreed on what house they're talking about.
rî'mâ | ʝel | mĭ-i | zym-zô. |
house | in.general | TOP-at | think-V.ACT |
I'm thinking about houses in general. [about the category of houses] |
Questions
Yes/no questions
Yes/no factual questions are formed by following the main verb with
{zǒn} (roughly equivalent to "ĉu" in Esperanto,
"-li"
in Volapük,
"-kah" in Malaysian,
etc.).
The questioned verb is often though not always fronted.
?ruŋ-zô | zǒn | ť | tu-i | pjylm-pwĭm-daj | o. |
go-V.ACT | Q.YN | 2 | AGT-at | border-water-mass | to |
Are you going to the beach?
Ordinarily the locative complement would come first, but questioning the verb
overrides this and shunts it to another position.
Questions with pairs of alternatives are formed using one of the
"or" conjunctions (ŝwiň, pwiň, hej, rej) and
placing the question particle {zǒn} just after the last of the
questioned alternatives.
?mjyl | rej | čĭm | zǒn | kâ-i | jyn-sra-van | ť | ʝâr-i. |
honey | or | chocolate | Q.YN | ATT-at | pleasure-COMP-V.STATE | 2 | EXP-at |
Do you get more pleasure from honey or chocolate?
Questions expecting a "yes" answer may insert {vǒm}
(yes, indeed, certainly) between the main verb and {zǒn};
similarly with questions expecting "no" and the negative
particle {heŋ}:
?mjyl | kâ-i | jyn-van | vǒm | zǒn | ť | ʝâr-i. |
honey | ATT-at | pleasure-V.STATE | yes | Q.YN | 2 | EXP-at |
You like honey, don't you?
?fĭm-cô-van | heŋ | zǒn | ƥ | ʝâr-i. |
health-OPP2-V.STATE | not | Q.YN | 3 | EXP-at |
She isn't sick, is she?
All of the aforementioned kinds of questions are usually answered with
{vǒm} (yes) or {heŋ} (no). However, {mwe} (necessary,
imperative) and {źǒ} (negative imperative) could be
used as emphatic forms in response to factual questions.
Questions asking for permission or advice either place the question
particle {zǒn} after an auxiliary verb,
?dlu-van | zǒn | ruŋ-zô | tyn | kǒ | ř. |
right-V.STATE | Q.YN | go-V.ACT | place | DEM1 | from |
May I be excused? |
...or use {mwe zǒn} or {źǒ zǒn} after the main verb,
?vlym | čâ-ja | ĥy-i | šâ-zô | mwe | zǒn. |
clothing | swim-suitable.for | PAT-at | carry-V.ACT | IMP | Q.YN |
Should I bring a swimsuit? |
?Φě'ĥu | ĥy-i | čĭn-Ł-zô | źǒ | zǒn. |
elephant | PAT-at | poke-3.GEN-V.ACT | IMP.NEG | Q.YN |
One shouldn't poke the elephant, should one? |
Questions asking someone for agreement with a proposed plan use the
question particle {srem}.
?Ќ | lĭw-o | rě'ĵy | jâ-o | srem | ť. |
1 | relation-to | wife | state-to | Q.YN.PLAN | 2 |
Will you marry me?
|
(Note that, as there is no verb in the above sentence, the question
particle follows the chief postpositional phrase.)
|
ðu-Ќ-van |
heŋ. |
*hum-ga-van |
gǒ |
ce. |
able-1-V.STATE |
not |
deep-METAPH-V.STATE |
behold |
this |
I can't. This is important. |
*sĭŋ-flu-kô |
i |
twâ-zô |
ĵlân-dô |
mâ |
sǒ. |
information-flow-place |
at |
say-V.ACT |
wisdom-violation |
person |
certain |
Someone is wrong on the Internet. |
|
Information questions
Information questions or question-word questions (who, what, where,
etc.) are formed with the particle {nǒ} following a general word for
the kind of entity whose nature is questioned, and followed by a
postposition for the role said unknown entity plays in the sentence;
the questionized postpositional phrase almost always comes first, even
if it would normally follow other consitutents in an indicative
sentence.
?mâ |
nǒ |
tu-i |
kĭlm |
o |
ruŋ-zô. |
person | Q.WH | AGT-at | party | to | go-V.ACT |
Who all is coming to the party? |
(Normal order of an indicative sentence would be locative, verb,
agent.)
?mâ |
nǒ |
mĭ-i |
fâ-ť-van. |
person | Q.WH | TOP-at | love-2-V.STATE |
Whom do you love? |
?tyn |
nǒ |
ř |
ruŋ-zô |
gâ-ŋa |
kǒ. |
place | Q.WH | from | come-V.ACT | thing-ATD5 | this |
Where did this thing come from? |
?ru |
nǒ |
i |
dĭn-ť-zô |
tǒ. |
manner |
Q.WH |
at |
knot-2-V.ACT |
that.2 |
How do you/did you tie that knot? |
?kujm |
nǒ |
o |
twâ-cu |
tǒ |
lju-θaj-ť-zô. |
goal |
Q.WH |
to |
sentence-system |
that.2 |
write-OPP1-2-V.ACT |
Why (for what purpose) are you writing that work? |
Note the way the cause and purpose question-phrases relate to
the corresponding postpositions {gân-ř} "because" and {kujm-o}
"for the purpose of", derived from the same roots.
?gâ |
nǒ |
syj-i |
te |
ĥy-i |
ķĭn-ƥ-zô. |
thing |
Q.WH |
use-at |
3.INAN |
PAT-at |
build-3-V.ACT |
What did he use to build it? |
?twâ-cu | nǒ | ĉul-i | hwâwm-ƥ-zô. |
sentence-system | Q.WH | perform-at | roleplaying-3-V.ACT |
What play are they performing? |
Some words take on a broader sense when used with the question-clitic
{nǒ} than they have in other contexts. For instance, {mâ} refers to
human persons, but {mâ nǒ} can question the identity of any animate
agent more generally; the speaker in the first three questions above
isn't necessarily assuming that the answer will refer to a human,
though it probably will. The most generic WH-question phrase is
{?gâ nǒ}, "thing which?"; but in most contexts a more specific
word would be selected; e.g.,
?gâ | nǒ | kâ-i | lju-ť-zô. |
thing | Q.WH | ATT-at | read-2-V.ACT |
What thing are you reading? |
is less likely than
?twâ-cu | nǒ | kâ-i | lju-ť-zô. |
sentence-system | Q.WH | ATT-at | read-2-V.ACT |
What written work are you reading? |
{gâ nǒ} is even a hypernym of {mâ nǒ}, in contexts where
the questioner is unsure whether the entity he's asking about
is even animate much less human.
The indefinite particle {kwǒ} is used along with {nǒ} to indicate
bewilderment or consternation:
?ru | nǒ | kwǒ | i | râ-van | ce. |
manner | Q.WH | some/any | at | event-V.STATE | this |
How the heck did this happen? |
?kujm | nǒ | kwǒ | o | žâ-ť-van. |
purpose | Q.WH | some/any | to | wait-2-V.STATE |
Why are you waiting? What are you waiting for? |
{zǒn} and more rarely {srem} can question other elements of a sentence besides the verb:
?mě'zâ | ĥy-i | zǒn | vâ-oŋ-zô | ť. |
corn | PAT-at | Q.YN | digestion-into-V.ACT | 2 |
Is that corn you're eating?
Note the difference from {nǒ}, and the different placement of these
particles relative to the postposition:
?mě'zâ | nǒ | ĥy-i | vâ-oŋ-zô | ť. |
corn | Q.WH | PAT-at | digestion-into-V.ACT | 2 |
Which corn [or, what kind of corn] are you eating?
Subordinate clauses
Subordinate clauses are introduced with {ðǒŋ} or
{hǒŋ} (like "that" or "whether" in English),
or one of the causal (because/therefore)
conjunctions ({wǒn}, {wǒj}, etc.).
{hǒŋ} is used to introduce subordinate clauses
which are the object of the main clause.
?kun-van | zǒn | ť | tu-i, | hǒŋ | tyn | kǒ | o | ruŋ-zô | ƥ | tu-i. |
know-V.STATE | Q.YN | 2 | AGT-at | that | place | DEM1 | to | go-V.ACT | 3 | AGT-at |
Did you know that he's coming?
Here, the subordinate clause introduced by {hǒŋ} is
the object of the main verb {kun-van} "know".
kun-van | heŋ, | hǒŋ | tyn | kǒ | o | ruŋ-zô | zǒn | ƥ | tu-i. |
know-V.STATE | not, | that | place | DEM1 | to | go-V.ACT | Q.YN | 3 | AGT-at |
I don't know whether she's coming.
Note the use of the question particle {zǒn} within the
subordinate clause; this makes {hǒŋ} mean "whether" instead
of "that". It works much the same with {srem} as well:
He asked her whether she would marry him.
The default subject in a {hǒŋ}-subordinate clause is the
subject of the main clause:
twâ-zô | tam-ram | tu-i, | hǒŋ | mwĭl-ŝra-van, | wǒn | ty | o | ruŋ-zô. |
say-V.ACT | Tom-NAME | AGT-at | that | sleep-about.to-V.STATE | therefore | home | to | go-V.ACT |
Tom said he was sleepy, so he was going home.
{ðǒŋ} is used to introduce subordinate clauses which are the
subject (typically the topic) of the main clause. A {ðǒŋ}
construction is equivalent to a similar construction with {hǒŋ}
having the forward-reference pronoun "že" in the main clause
as a dummy subject (like "it" in similar senteces in English).
is equivalent to:
Relative clauses
In a relative clause, the relativizer {lǒ} is placed after a noun phrase
referring to the relativized element in the main clause, and is followed by a
spacetime or case postposition as appropriate for its role in the relative
clause. The relativized element comes first in the relative clause, whatever
its role. Any element in the main clause can be relativized, as well as
non-overt elements (e.g., time or place where a main clause doesn't have an
overtly expressed temporal or locative complement). And the relativized element
can have any role in the relative clause.
Relative clauses come after the main clause; they do not embed in it like
in English.
The noun phrase that is relativized doesn't have to be fully repeated in the
relative clause; the noun phrase preceding {lǒ} just has recognizably refer
to a unique element of the main clause. (E.g., usually {mâ} is used when the
relativized element refers to the only person mentioned in the matrix clause,
but if more than one person was mentioned, some other noun (probably a {mâ-}
based compound) is used instead.)
Relativized element is object of main clause and subject of the relative clause:
gě'dĭm-zla |
hǒl |
θje |
il |
pĭw-zô |
kǒ |
de. |
sleep.wake.cycle-COLL |
whole |
almost |
through |
play-V.ACT |
DEM1 |
HAB |
I played chess with Elizabeth, who had been playing almost her whole life. |
Relativized element is locative complement of main clause and object of relative
clause:
tyn |
lǒ |
ĥy-i |
ħulŋ-źa-Ł-zô |
θǒ. |
place |
REL |
PAT-at |
damage-AUG-3.GEN-V.ACT |
just |
I once stayed in that old hotel they just demolished. |
Relativized element is not overtly present in main clause:
Note that the equivalents of some of these relative clauses would not be
considered relative clauses in English, but maybe complement clauses introduced
by "when", "while", "where" etc.
If you have multiple relative clauses in a sentence, each comes at the end of
the clause whose element it relativizes.
zuň-cô-fwa-zô, |
mâ-ŝy |
lǒ |
tu-i |
hĭj-rě'ĵy-dô-zô. |
life-OPP2-CAUS-V.ACT |
person-female |
REL |
AGT-at |
sacrament-wife-violation-V.ACT |
The man who murdered his wife who cheated on him was executed today. |
If two elements in the main clause are relativized, they can come in either
order after the main clause.
mâ |
lǒ |
tu-i |
lě'jâ |
rjâ-i |
rě'ju-kâj-zô. |
person |
REL |
AGT-at |
olive |
quest-to |
search-exchange-V.ACT |
I met Sarah, who was shopping for olives, when I went to the store for butter. |
Note that because a relative clause starts with a noun phrase followed by {lǒ},
not with a conjunction like subordinate clauses, you can't omit a subject case
postposition after an overt subject in the main clause.
The main clause can have a cataphoric {ɱ} or {ŋe} pronoun pointing forward to
the relativized element of the relative clause.
ŝĭw |
lǒ |
ĥy-i |
bîn'zĭ-ĵwa |
i |
kâj-zô |
kyn-ŝâm-la. |
substance |
REL |
PAT-at |
gasoline-place |
at |
exchange-V.ACT |
parent-womb-ATD1 |
I rubbed my back with this stuff Mom bought at a gas station to stop the pain. |
Conditional clauses
The conjunctions {ĉǒ} and {bǒ} can both be glossed as
"if", and they introduce the
protasis
of a conditional statement.
The apodosis can be
introduced by any of the "therefore" conjunctions
{ŝǒn, ʝǒn, wǒn} as appropriate — most
often {wǒn}.
{bǒ} is used for conditions within the speaker's control,
referring to uncertain plans and future actions. E.g.,
bǒ |
jork-wam-bâm |
o |
ruŋ-źa-zô, |
wǒn |
|
if |
York-NAME.P-new |
to |
go-AUG-V.ACT |
then |
ħĭn-ta-tǒj |
slân-i |
Φâ |
kâ-i |
rĭm-van. |
restriction-without-NMZ |
symbol-at |
statue |
ATT-at |
see-V.STATE |
If I go to New York, I'll see the Statue of Liberty. |
{ĉǒ} is used for conditions beyond the speaker's control, whether
unknown past events, hypothetical future events, or counterfactuals.
ĉǒ |
bly-van |
pwĭm, |
wǒn |
re |
o |
ruŋ-zô |
heŋ |
gwî'jum. |
if |
fall-V.STATE |
water |
then |
3.PLACE |
to |
go-V.ACT |
not |
William |
If it rains, William won't go [to the aforementioned place]. |
The particle {šelm} is used after the verb or the main postpositional
phrase in the apodosis, if (a) the protasis is counterfactual, or (b)
the protasis is implied and omitted.
ĉǒ |
čĭm |
ŋĭn-i |
pwĭm, |
wǒn |
ĝâj-van |
šelm |
ĥâ-ŋĭw-zla. |
if |
chocolate |
CMT-at |
water |
then |
decay-V.STATE |
HYPOTH |
cut-organ-COLL |
If water were chocolate, all of the teeth would decay. |
bǒ |
pwĭm-pul-daj |
iŋ |
pĭw-zô |
vlym-ta, |
if |
water-powder-COLL |
inside |
play-V.ACT |
clothes-without |
wǒn |
fĭm-cô |
jâ-o |
šelm |
bî'duň |
pě'ŝlĭ-i. |
then |
health-OPP2 |
state-to |
HYPOTH |
sinuses |
focus-at |
If I were to play naked in the snow (which I won't do), I'd catch a cold. |
The latter use often occurs in a reply to someone's proposal; e.g., if
Tim says:
?Ќ |
ty-i |
kĭlm-zô |
srem. |
1 |
home-at |
party-V.ACT |
Q.YN.PLAN |
Shall we have a party at my house? |
then Manuel might reply:
ce |
mĭ-i |
pwĭ-van |
šelm. |
this |
TOP-at |
delight-V.ACT |
HYPOTH |
I would be delighted [if we did]. |
This apodosis with implied protasis is also sometimes used
following an imperative, especially a negative imperative, giving a
reason for the command or request.
pwĭm-pul |
iŋ |
pĭw-ť-zô |
vlym-ta |
źǒ. |
water-powder-COLL |
inside |
play-2-V.ACT |
clothes-without |
NEG.IMP |
fĭm-cô |
jâ-o |
šelm. |
health-OPP2 |
state-to |
HYPOTH |
Don't play naked in the snow. You would get sick. |
mě'hu |
gǒ |
ĥy-i |
vâ-oŋ-ť-zô |
mwe. |
stew |
behold |
PAT-at |
digestion-into-2-V.ACT |
IMP |
te |
kâ-i |
jyn-lym-van |
šelm. |
3.INAN |
ATT-at |
pleasure-taste-V.STATE |
HYPOTH |
Here, eat this stew! You would find it tasty. |
{dlen} introduces an "or else" or "otherwise" clause. Typically
it would come after the {wǒn} clause, at the end of a conditional
construction:
dlen |
ty |
iŋ |
ŝě'ĥâ-môj |
tu-mrân-bô. |
otherwise |
home |
inside |
chess-V.RECP |
AGT-relaxing.meal-ADJ |
If it's sunny, we'll picnic in the park, otherwise we'll play chess while eating at home. |
{dlen} can also introduce a clause presenting the alternative if a command or piece of
advice is not obeyed.
dlen |
wĭm-ŝum-da |
jâ-o |
še |
te |
ĥy-i |
Ł |
tu-i. |
otherwise |
sack-float-full |
state-to |
maybe |
3.INAN |
PAT-at |
3.GEN |
AGT-at |
|
Lock your car in this neighborhood, or else someone might fill it with balloons. |
Miscellaneous modifier particles
Temporal particles
mje | past |
ler | future |
de | nowadays; lately; (with {mje}) in those days (habitual aspect, extended tense) |
gwe | already (now in contrast with recent past) |
ver | still, yet (now as similar to recent past) |
{mje} and {ler} were discussed briefly in the Verbs section. They are not used
like tense marking in English and other Indo-European languages, obligatory on
every finite verb, but tend to be used only when the time of an event is not
otherwise clear from context. Often, {mje} can function kind of like a perfect
aspect in an Indo-European language, indicating that something happened before
the surrounding context (which may already be in the past from the speaker or
listener's perspective).
gjâ-krĭ |
ĵyn-fwa-ĵar |
srǒ |
krĭ-o |
byn-zô |
mje. |
language-create |
intellectual.pleasure-CAUS-less |
several |
create-at |
hack-V.ACT |
PAST |
I had created several less interesting languages (before that). |
Here, {mje} indicates that the second sentence takes place in the past relative
to the first.
{ler} is used more often than {mje}, but not as often as {gwe} and {ver}
(discussed below). Besides the basic meaning of "this verb's action will take
place in the future," it can also occur after an event-noun or time-period noun
phrase and indicate that it is the next, not the previous, instance that is
meant:
One could also write the same sentence with {ler} after {jâ-o} instead.
{de} is glossed as HAB = habitual aspect; it indicates that something happens
(perhaps intermittently, perhaps continually) over an extended period of time.
Whether it's translated as "nowadays, lately" or "in those days" depends on
the temporal context (set by postpositional phrases, {mje}, or {ler}).
ĥwĭl |
pǒ |
i |
ewropa-wam |
i |
râ-van |
de |
sî'ðyr-źa-cu. |
era |
that |
at |
Europe-NAME.P |
at |
happen-V.STATE |
HAB |
fight-AUG-system |
Wars happened continually/intermittently in Europe at that period. |
I use {ver} and {gwe} more often than any of the above particles. They indicate
change or continuity in an action or process in implicit comparison to a past time.
flyr-da |
jâ-i |
ver |
kěr'nâ. |
flower-full |
STATE-at |
still |
dogwood |
The dogwood is still in bloom. |
mâ | ʝǒ | tu-i | mwe | vě'ty-rĭm | ble | ĥy-i | šyj-zô. |
person | other | AGT-at | IMP | door-seeing | others | PAT-at | wash-V.ACT |
I've already washed seventeen windows. Someone else will have to wash the rest. |
"not yet" and "no longer" are also expressed with {ver} and {gwe} in ways that
might be counterintuitive at first.
źĭ |
lǒ |
kâ-i |
vy-zô |
kun-hôw-ca |
heŋ |
ver |
sě'râ. |
field.of.study |
REL |
ATT-at |
decide-V.ACT |
know-CAUS-REFL |
not |
still |
Sarah |
Sarah hasn't yet decided what field to study. |
Dhalgren-gam |
kâ-i |
lju-zô |
heŋ |
ver. |
Dhalgren-NAME.G |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
not |
still |
I still haven't / haven't yet read Dhalgren. |
{ver} and {gwe} are glossed with respect to "now" in the table above, but they
can compare a past or future time with a time before the referenced time.
źî'câ-gla |
i |
gjâ |
kǒ |
krĭ-i |
byn-gĭn-zô |
heŋ |
ver. |
1997-ORD.T |
at |
language |
this |
create-at |
hack-begin-V.ACT |
not |
still |
In 1997 I had not yet begun to hack this language. |
nu |
tǒ |
mĭ-i |
nu |
kujm-ja |
šir |
gwe. |
moment |
DEM2 |
TOP-at |
moment |
goal-suitable |
long.after |
already |
By the time you read this, it will already be much too late. |
Epistemic particles
There are several clitic particles that change the truth-value
with which a phrase or clause is intended. Like other modifiers,
they're postpositive, typically coming after the main verb of the
sentence or the most verblike postpositional phrase, although they
can also come after other sentence constituents. They
can also stand alone as utterances responding to a question.
vǒm | yes, certainly |
heŋ | no, not |
fjǒ | yes and no; sort of; to some degree |
še | maybe [facts] |
be | maybe [intentions] |
le | probably |
ʝem | apparently, seemingly |
nen | not as far as the speaker knows |
tǒlm | hyperbole/exaggeration marker |
belm | irony or sarcasm marker |
These express one's certainty about something being true or false,
real or unreal. {še} expresses uncertainty whether some
statement or description is true or not. {fjǒ} expresses a
belief that a sentence or description is valid to some degree, but not totally.
{be} indicates an uncertainty of one's own plans or intentions.
bĭŋ-van | vǒm. |
exist-V.STATE | yes |
I certainly exist.
mwĭl-van | heŋ. |
sleep-V.STATE | not |
I'm not asleep.
kyl | iŋ | râm | mĭ-i | zuň-cô-bô | še | ŋĭn-i. |
box | inside | cat | TOP-at | life-OPP2-ADJ | maybe | CMT-at |
The cat in the box might be dead.
fĭw-câŋ |
fjǒ |
ŋĭn-i |
θuň-lâŋ |
pǒ. |
fiction-experimental.science |
FUZZY |
CMT-at |
story-long |
that |
That novel is sort of science fiction. |
In the above sentences, the particles modify a whole verb or comment
phrase. They can also be clitic'd to a word and used within a phrase.
kjĭ | ĥy-i | vâ-oŋ-zô | râm-vǒm | tu-i. |
mouse | PAT-at | digestion-into-V.ACT | cat-yes | AGT-at |
The definitely-cat eats a mouse.
{le} following the main verb of the sentence affects the truth-value
of the sentence as a whole; following a specific noun phrase, it
affects only its referent. For instance,
{ʝem} likewise can follow the main verb, expressing reservations
about the situation described by the sentence as a whole, or
follow another sentence constituent and modify only that:
{nen} is a reserved, hesitant {heŋ}; it signifies that the speaker
doesn't know of any particular evidence for a proposition, but won't
assert that it's not true.
fĭm-cô-ƥ-van |
nen. |
health-OPP2-3-V.STATE | NEG.UNCERTAIN |
She's not sick as far as I know. |
?ðu-ť-van |
zǒn |
pjân-zô. |
able-2-V.STATE | Q.YN | piano-V.ACT |
Can you play the piano? |
nen |
belm. |
NEG.UNCERTAIN | IRONY |
Not that I know of. |
See also the
section on derived validationality adverbs.
The derived validational adverb {ĵrĭw-pôm}, from the root
{ĵrĭw} "expecting, supposing", is something like a positive
equivalent of {nen}: "As far as I know..." or "I suppose but don't have
strong evidence that..."
Expressing probability and fuzzy degree of truth
A
fractional number
between zero and one, marked with the {-bô}
adjectival suffix, can modify one of the clitics {be, še, le, fjǒ}.
firence-wam |
o |
ruŋ-zô |
ler |
be |
ðe-dâ-lwa-bô. |
Firenze-NAME.P | to | go-V.ACT | FUT | maybe | divided.by-three-APPROX-ADJ |
There's about a one-third chance I will go to Florence. |
nu |
lǒ |
i |
lâl-van |
gju-ŋy-ĉa. |
time |
REL |
at |
noise-V.STATE |
speak-far-tool |
She had gotten a third of the way dressed when the phone rang. |
Irony and Hyperbole
gjâ-zym-byn does not
use tone to mark ironic or
sarcastic remarks (or for any other purpose); instead, the particle
{belm} is used, typically following the verb (like the yes/no question
particles {zǒn} and {srem}) or at the end of a sentence, but
sometimes marking an ironically intended noun phrase. The last use
could sometimes be translated by the phrase "so-called" or
the use of scare quotes.
Ќ | cim | šâ-ŋĭw | ĥy-i | vâ-oŋ-zô | ₣âl-bô | rjuŋ. |
1 | left.part.of | carry-organ | PAT-at | digestion-into-V.ACT | sudden-ADJ | dragon |
"How delightful!" I said as the dragon chomped off my left arm and hand. |
sĭŋ-flu-kô | ki | lju-θaj-zô. |
information-flow-place | throughout | read-OPP1-V.ACT |
A certain "friend" of hers wrote about her secrets all over the Internet. |
{belm} can also be used for ironic understatement, e.g.,
vâm-plâŋ-zô |
šelm |
Φě'ĥu. |
crush-foot-V.ACT |
HYPOTH |
elephant |
It would not be convenient to get trampled by an elephant. |
{tǒlm} marks deliberate exaggeration or hyperbole.
gě'dĭm |
hǒl |
il |
tǒlm |
gjâ |
kǒ |
mĭ-i |
mî'ħâ-van. |
day | whole | during | HYPERBOLE | language | DEM1 | TOP-at | obsess-V.STATE |
I've been obsessed with this language all day [but not literally every moment thereof]. |
kî'pĭ |
ky-bô |
tǒlm |
ŝĭw-i |
mě'hu |
ķĭn-o. |
pepper | thirty.seven-ADJ | HYPERBOLE | material-at | stew | making-to |
I put thirty-seven peppers in this stew [no, not really that many, but a lot]. |
Deontic particles
mwe | imperative, hortative, desiderative particle
|
źǒ | negative version of {mwe}
|
{mwe} and {źǒ} are glossed as IMP = imperative and IMP.NEG = negative
imperative, but in fact their use is broader than the imperative in English.
They can express a wish, desire, advice or request as well as a command, and
they don't cause verbs to default to having a second person subject. They can occur with any
subject or an omitted default subject.
gjâ-zym-byn has a verb {dlâw-van} which can be glossed as "should, must, ought
to," but it's used less often than its English equivalents; instead of
structuring a sentence like "dlâw-van [second verb] [subject]", gzb would often
use "[verb] mwe [subject]".
ť | ŋâw-o | twâ-zô | mwe | ce | mĭ-i. |
2 | call-to | say-V.ACT | IMP | that | TOP-at |
I should tell you about that.
re |
o |
ruŋ-ť-zô |
źǒ. |
3.PLACE |
to |
go-2-V.ACT |
IMP.NEG |
Don't go there. |
suŋ-kě'ĝu-tla | ŋâw-o | twâ-zô | Φǒ | {*Φĭlm-van | źǒ.} |
know.how-secret-professional | call-to | say-V.ACT | QUOTE | butterfly-V.STATE | IMP.NEG |
"I don't want to be a butterfly!" I said to the sorcerer.
kiň | θě'ku | tu-i | twâ-zô | Φǒ | {bĭŋ-van | mwe | fu.}, | kiň | bĭŋ-van | fu. |
and | God | AGT-at | say-V.ACT | QUOTE | exist-V.STATE | IMP | light | and | exist-V.STATE | light |
And God said, "Let there be light", and there was light.
brâl-van, | hǒŋ | twâ-zô | mwe | le | hǒŋ | še | vǒm |
be.certain-V.STATE | that | say-V.ACT | IMP | probably | that | maybe | yes |
I am positive that a definite maybe is probably in order. |
{mwe} and {źǒ} can occur after a postposition in a verbless sentence:
{źǒ} can occur following a noun phrase in at least one construction:
Here, the {źǒ}-marked noun phrase as an object of {rjâ-i} means one is
searching for something one hopes not to find. Similar constructions are used
for e.g., checking one's boots for scorpions or someone's blood sample for signs
of disease. I'm not sure if {mwe} or {źǒ} could occur following noun phrases
otherwise.
{źǒ} is often used in subordinate clauses introduced with {kujm-šar}, "in
order to," to negate the sense of the conjunction, "in order to
avoid/prevent/stop something."
{mwe} occurs less often in these contexts because the {kujm-šar} already
implies something that someone desires to happen.
kujm-šar |
suomi-wam |
o |
ruŋ-źa-zô. |
purpose-CONJ |
Finland-NAME.P |
to |
go-AUG-V.ACT |
Thomas is teaching himself Finnish in order to travel to Finland. |
Postposition-like adverbs
gǒ | presentative marker |
hǒ | vocative marker |
jej | enthusiasm marker |
These are anomalous particles which have the form of {jum}, but can
act like postpositions; they can mark a noun phrase so as to stand
alone as a valid sentence by itself, and sometimes when they mark a
constituent of a larger sentence it may not require another
case-postposition. {gǒ} is a presentative particle, corresponding to
French "voici"
or "voilá", archaic English "lo" or "behold", or
Esperanto "jen",
drawing the listener's attention to something.
te |
gǒ. |
3.INAN | behold |
Here it is. |
vĭn |
gǒ. |
wine | behold |
Look, [I brought] wine. |
A noun phrase followed by {gǒ}, as a stand-alone sentence, can
function like a topic-comment sentence; this structure tends to be
more emphatic and informal than the topic-comment or comment-topic
form.
kyr-ta |
ŋĭn-i |
twâ |
kǒ. |
verb-less | CMT-at | sentence | DEM1 |
This sentence [is] verbless. |
twâ |
kyr-ta |
gǒ. |
sentence | verb-less | behold |
Look, a sentence with no verb. |
When it marks a constituent of a larger sentence, {gǒ} can be a kind
of topicalizer (in a different sense of "topic" than that with which
it's used for the gzb "topic postposition" {mĭ-i}).
mwĭl-ŝra-van |
gǒ |
râm. |
sleep-tending-V.STATE | behold | cat |
See how sleepy the cat is. |
pân |
ĥy-i |
gǒ |
bâm-fwa-zô. |
everything | PAT-at | behold | new-CAUS-V.ACT |
Behold, I make all things new. |
{hǒ} is the vocative marker, following the second-person pronoun, a
person's name, or another appellation when one begins an utterance
addressed to them; it's not to be confused with the
object-of-communication case postposition {ŋâw-o}, which marks
someone's name (etc.) as the addressee of a communication-verb:
The vocative particle can mark a name or other appellation as a
stand-alone utterance, when one's just calling someone to get their
attention and not immediately saying something in particular to them:
A conventional greeting uses these two particles together with
the first and second person pronouns:
ť |
hǒ, |
Ќ |
gǒ. |
2 | VOC | 1 | behold |
Hello. [literally: O you, behold me.] |
{jej} is vaguely similar in meaning and use to the English
interjection from which its form is borrowed. It can work like {gǒ},
marking a noun phrase which thus stands alone as a valid utterance:
Or it can mark a constituent within a larger sentence; in this use it
overlaps in meaning with the affectionate and respectful attitudinal
suffixes, but tends to express a more excited, enthusiastic attitude;
it can in fact be combined with an attitudinal suffix.
*gâm-ʝĭl |
pǒ |
gân-ř |
kě'pâ |
jâ-o |
jej. |
picture-motion | DEM3 | cause-from | happy.bewilderment | state-to | yay |
Wow, that film has put me in a state of happy bewilderment! |
These adverbs can also function as interjections, as stand-alone
utterances:
*gǒ.
Look!
*hǒ.
Hey!
*jej.
Yay!
Qualifiers
Most of these particles describe the degree to which some adjective or
verb is applicable. Some can also be used with noun phrases.
θje | almost; not quite |
fem | at least, anyway |
ƴeŋ | barely, hardly |
fe | slightly, a little bit |
źe | very, very much, a lot |
ķe | too, too much |
žǒŋ | only, merely, simply, just |
mew | even, also, too |
gem | especially, particularly |
jǒj | again, more, still |
mǒj | te | ðim | du | fem | mĭ-i | ĵyn-fwa-van. |
but | 3.INAN | early.part.of | chapter | at.least | TOP-at | interest-CAUS-V.STATE |
Your story is not quite worth reading, but its first chapter at least is interesting. |
te | im | du | ĉu-pa | mew | im | mâ-fĭw | pym-fwa | gem. |
3.INAN | part.of | chapter | two-ORD | also | part.of | person-fiction | amusement-CAUS | especially |
In the second chapter too there are some especially funny characters. |
mǒj | du | dâ-pa | mĭ-i | hî'mâr-faj | ƴeŋ | pe | zâň-bô | ķe | ŋĭn-i. |
but | chapter | three-ORD | TOP-at | understand-able | barely | and | idiosyncratic | too.much | CMT-at |
But the third chapter is barely comprehensible and too idiosyncratic. |
{mew} "also, too" can come after any number of sentence constituents. Depending
on the intended meaning, it can come after a noun phrase in subject position
with no overt postposition, or after the postposition of a normal postpositional
phrase, or after a verb.
kĭlm |
o |
ruŋ-zô |
mew |
θě'mâ. |
party |
to |
go-V.ACT |
too |
Thomas |
Thomas also came to the party (in addition to other things he did). |
kĭlm |
o |
ruŋ-zô |
θě'mâ |
mew. |
party |
to |
go-V.ACT |
Thomas |
too |
Thomas, too, came to the party (in addition to other party guests). |
kĭlm |
o |
mew |
ruŋ-zô |
θě'mâ. |
party |
to |
too |
go-V.ACT |
Thomas |
Thomas came to the party, too (in addition to other places he went). |
If the subject pronoun is incorporated into the verb, it can be ambiguous
as to whether {mew} is intended to modify the subject or the verb.
mluŋ-čĭm |
ĥy-i |
šâ-ƥ-zô |
mew. |
bread-chocolate |
PAT-at |
carry-3-V.ACT |
too |
He also brought chocolate cookies. / He, too, brought chocolate cookies. |
Quantifiers
These are typically used as nonspecific quantifiers with noun phrases.
When used to modify a verb, they (like number-adjectives in the same
context) mean the action is done so many times.
zen | only, no more than; no one but; (when not qualifying a number) single, sole, alone |
kwǒ | some, any |
srǒ | several |
cǒ | few, little |
reŋ | much, many, a lot |
gle | enough, sufficient |
jǒj | more, extra; again |
pen | all, every, each |
hǒl | whole, entire |
jǒm | most of, the majority of |
ðǒl | mostly |
Note the contrast between {cǒ} (few, little) and {fe} (little,
slightly). {cǒ} modifies noun phrases, whether count or
noncount: {rî'zĭ cǒ}, a little rice, {θĭl
cǒ}, a few potatoes. {fe} modifies verb phrases or adjectives,
indicating that the action is done to a lesser than usual degree or in
a casual, haphazard way, or for a shorter than usual time, or that the
quality is present in a limited degree. {cǒ} used with verbs
means that the action is done a few times or for a short time. There
can be some overlap in their meaning vis-a-vis verbs. {reŋ} is
similarly indifferent to count/mass distinctions: {lî'klâ
reŋ}, a lot of milk; {cî'jyr reŋ}, many squirrels. (In
gzb there is little or no real distinction between count and noncount
nouns as in English, since {cǒ} and {reŋ} translate both
"few/little" and "many/much".)
Compare {fe} with the fuzzy logic clitic {fjǒ}:
gâm-ʝil | kǒ | kâ-i | rĭm-van | fjǒ. |
picture-motion | DEM1 | ATT-at | see-V.STATE | FUZZY |
I am sort of watching this movie. |
gâm-ʝil | kǒ | kâ-i | rĭm-van | fe. |
picture-motion | DEM1 | ATT-at | see-V.STATE | slightly |
I watched part of this movie. |
{jǒj}, "more, again" is a bit polysemous, especially with verbs:
could mean, "I read some more of Luke's Gospel" or "I read Luke's Gospel
again"; but probably the latter. For the former sense one would more likely
say {lju-zô jǒj fe}.
{kwǒ} is similarly polysemous, but not as often ambiguous.
?ť | wuŋ-i | zǒn | te | mĭ-i. |
2 | ownership-at | Q.YN | 3.INAN | TOP-at |
I found somebody's parasol. Is it yours? |
The particles {zen} and {žǒŋ} are both glossed as "only". {zen} is
a quantitative "only"; it is used in sentences like "Only Tom came to
the party", "We have only four bananas left", and so forth. {žǒŋ}
could also be translated "merely" in many cases; for instance, when a
door has opened apparently by itself, you might say,
{ķe} describes an excess of a quality or action; to say there are too
many of something, use {reŋ ķe}. (I may change this.) "Too little" (quality)
and "too few" (quantity) are {ķe-cô} and {cǒ ķe}.
ƥ | pen | ĥy-i | ðu-van | heŋ | šu-zô |
3.AN | all | PAT-at | able-V.STATE | not | take.care.of-V.ACT |
You have too many cats; you can't take care of them all. |
Ќ | ŋâw-o | nî'šĭm | ĝy-i | gju-ŋy-ť-zô | reŋ | ķe. |
1 | call-to | night | middle-at | speak-distant-2-V.ACT | many | too.much |
You've telephoned me too many times in the middle of the night. |
Some of these nonspecific quantifiers can modify another quantifier or
a number-adjective.
ƥ | ty-i | ty-van | râm | fy-bô | gle | belm. |
3 | home-at | home-V.STATE | cat | seven-ADJ | enough | IRONY |
Seven cats — enough, I think — live with him. |
fy | jǒm | ʝâr-i | kjĭ | mĭ-i | ħum-van. |
seven | most.of | experiencer-at | mouse | TOP-at | fear-V.STATE |
Most of the seven are afraid of mice. |
(Note that here {fy}, "seven", is the head of a noun-phrase rather
than an adjective as {fy-bô} in the previous sentence; in such a
context it doesn't represent the mathematical object "seven" but
rather a salient, recently mentioned set of seven things.)
{jǒm} and {ðǒl}
The modifiers {jǒm} and {ðǒl} have similar but distinct meanings.
{jǒm} indicates a subset, a set consisting of most of the members
of a superset denoted by the head noun phrase.
twâ-cu jǒm | most of the books
|
tu-gu jǒm | most of the voters, a majority
|
{ðǒl} on the other hand indicates a superset, mostly consisting of
members of the set denoted by the noun phrase modified by {ðǒl}. By
contrast, you might use {twâ-cu ðǒl}, "mostly books", to describe a
box of heterogeneous things including books, clothes, dishes, and
ceramic statues of frogs, or {tu-gu ðǒl}, "mostly voters", to
describe the set of people you polled on some political question.
These minimal pair sentences may contrast their meanings more clearly:
ĝâk-ram |
vâns-šam |
dâm-ř |
twâ-cu |
jǒm |
kâ-i |
lju-zô. |
Jack-NAME |
Vance-NAME.F |
author-from |
sentence-system |
most.of |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
I've read most of Jack Vance's works. |
ĝâk-ram |
vâns-šam |
dâm-ř |
twâ-cu |
ðǒl |
kâ-i |
lju-zô. |
Jack-NAME |
Vance-NAME.F |
author-from |
sentence-system |
mostly |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
I'm mostly reading stuff by Jack Vance. |
In the latter sentence, one is not asserting that one has read the
majority of Vance's works, but is implying that one is also reading
some other works by people other than Vance.
{jǒm} always means at least a 50%+1 majority, and may pragmatically
suggest a fairly large majority; {ðǒl} need not indicate a majority,
only a plurality — perhaps you might utter the second sentence above
if four of the last ten books you'd read were by Jack Vance, and you'd
read no more than three by any other author in this
{drulm}.
{jǒm} and {ðǒl} can modify postpositional phrases as well.
mwĭl |
jâ-o |
jǒm |
gwe |
râm. |
sleep |
state-to |
most.of |
already |
cat |
The cat has mostly fallen asleep. |
ĝâk-ram |
vâns-šam |
₣um-i |
ðǒl |
mě'θâj |
hjuz-šam. |
Jack-NAME |
Vance-NAME.F |
similar-to |
mostly |
Matthew |
Hughes-NAME.F |
Matthew Hughes mostly resembles Jack Vance (= his style resembles Vance's more than that of any other author) |
In some cases, {jǒm} or {ðǒl} following a postpostion can be
interpreted as modifying an implicit noun phrase between themselves
and the postpositional phrase; for instance,
ĝâk-ram |
vâns-šam |
dâm-ř |
jǒm |
kâ-i |
lju-zô. |
Jack-NAME |
Vance-NAME.F |
author-from |
most.of |
ATT-at |
read-V.ACT |
I've read most of Jack Vance's (works). |
{jǒm} also has a qualitative sense, usually when it's used with
one modifier in contrast with another; for instance,
With units of measure, {jǒm} means "more than half, less than one":
{ðǒl} with verbs means that a plurality or majority of the speaker's
time is spent on the action described by the verb:
gě'dĭm |
kǒ |
i |
gjâ |
kǒ |
ĥy-i |
byn-zô |
ðǒl. |
day |
DEM1 |
at |
language |
DEM1 |
PAT-at |
tinker-V.ACT |
mostly |
Today I'm mostly tinkering with this language. (= that is how a plurality of my time is spent) |
tâŋ |
pǒ |
i |
ðurm |
ðĭ-i |
šĭm-cu-vuj |
kâ-i |
ħĭ-zô |
ðǒl |
de. |
life.era |
DEM3 |
at |
work |
relation-at |
algorithm-system-concrete |
ATT-at |
test-V.ACT |
mostly |
HAB |
In those days my work mostly involved software testing. |
{jǒm} means that the ongoing action of the verb is mostly completed,
or that the now-completed action of a verb was mostly done within a
certain time-frame. It might be combined with other aspectual adverbs
or a temporal complement phrase for clarity.
źě'mâ-ky-gla |
i |
θuň-lâŋ |
pǒ |
krĭ-o |
lju-θaj-zô |
jǒm. |
1973-37-ORD.T |
at |
story-long |
DEM3 |
create-at |
read-OPP1-V.ACT |
most.of |
In 2010 I wrote the bulk of that novel (but perhaps started it earlier and/or finished it later). |
{ðǒl} is a relatively late addition to the language; in older
texts {jǒm} is sometimes used in the same sense {ðǒl} has now.
Error correction particles
There are two error correction particles, used for correcting an
utterance partway through when one realizes one has made a mistake.
{Φej} deletes the previous word (tells the listener to please
ignore it), allowing one to utter some other word in its place.
{če} is a conjunction that reverses the normal word order
between its two arguments: so ADJ če NOUN is acceptable
though NOUN ADJ would be the normal correct order. Also related
is the hesitation particle {hem}, corresponding to "uh, um"
in English: "I haven't thought of the next word yet, but
I'm not done talking, be patient."
bě'gru | Φej | bě'jâ | řm | ʝym | mĭ-i | šul | jyn-fwa | ŋĭn-i. |
beaver | excuse.me | bay.tree | out.of | leaf | TOP-at | spice | pleasure-CAUS | CMT-at |
Beaver, I mean bay, leaves are a good spice. |
suw-fwa | če | Φě'ĥu-bâm | tu-i |
enjoyment.of.cuteness-CAUS | REV | elephant-new | AGT-at |
ƴâ-cjaj-zô, | hem, | flâň-bô. |
move.under.control-SPEC-V.ACT | um | shaky-ADJ |
The cute baby elephant walks, um, sort of wobblily.
(I stole the idea for the particle {Φej} from Jeffrey Henning's
Fith, a stack-based
language that has particles which simply pop the top item, or all
items, from the stack and discard them. Unlike the
Fith conjunctions frong or skuunh, however,
{Φej} can't be expected to make the listener really forget the
previous word, since gjâ-zym-byn is a human language and the
language center of human brains doesn't work that way. So it's not
used to weaken severe insults into mild ones like the corresponding
Fith particles, but simply to ask pardon for slips of the tongue, as
above.)
I use {Φej} a lot when talking to myself in gzb,
and not infrequently in writing. {hem}, not so much.
{hem hem hem} is roughly equivalent to "blah blah blah"
in English, filler to represent speech or text whose exact content
is unknown or unimportant.
Various other modifier particles
ʝǒ | other, another |
ble | others, rest of, remainder of |
θǒ | immediate, next, previous |
sǒ | certain, specific |
rew | apiece, each |
mrel | discourse marker indicating resumption of a dormant topic |
men | on the one hand... on the other hand... |
ŝe | in contrast, by contrast, on the contrary |
sem | so, therefore, thus |
{ʝǒ} following a noun phrase indicates a different instance of the referent of
the noun phrase than was previously mentioned, or than is about to be mentioned.
ƥ |
ŋâw-o |
ħâň-zô |
ƴâw |
ʝǒ. |
3 |
call-to |
yell-V.ACT |
dog |
another |
I went for a walk with my dog. Another dog barked at her. |
srǒ |
krĭ-o |
lju-θaj-zô |
lě'kjân. |
several |
create-at |
read-OPP-V.ACT |
Alex |
Alex has written several novels in collaboration with several other authors. |
{ble} indicates that, in addition to the referent of the previous noun phrase,
all the other entities in the relevant category are intended.
źy |
ble |
kâ-i |
hyw-van |
heŋ |
gwe. |
dream |
rest.of |
ATT-at |
know-V.STATE |
not |
already |
I no longer remember the rest of (the night's) dreams. |
[From a journal entry, just after an account of one dream I remembered well.]
{θǒ} with a noun phrase denoting a time-period or event can mean
either "next" or "previous" depending on context. If context is
otherwise insufficient, {θǒ} is often used along with {mje} (past)
or {ler} (future). Modifying a verb, {θǒ} means "immediately, right
away" or sometimes "suddenly."
ĥun | θǒ | i, | šâ-ť-zô | mwe | twâ-cu | pǒ |
ĥy-i. |
meeting | next/prev | at | carry-2-V.ACT | IMP | sentence-system | DEM3 |
PAT-at |
Bring that book to [our] next meeting. |
nu-šar | ŝrun-twâ-gĭn-zô | θǒ. |
moment-CONJ | music-sentence-begin-V.ACT | immediate |
He drank one glass of wine, then immediately started singing. |
{rew} can be glossed as "apiece," though it has a broader use than "apiece" in
English. It indicates a distribution of something to multiple targets in some way.
Unlike in English, it marks the targets of distribution, not the thing distributed.
(In older texts, its use was inconsistent, sometimes marking the target and sometimes
the thing distributed, and sometimes coming after the noun phrase, sometimes after
the postposition.)
ty |
rew |
o |
ruŋ-zô |
mâ |
pen. |
home |
apiece |
to |
go-V.ACT |
person |
all |
Everyone went to their separate homes. |
Here, the things distributed are the agent.
Here, the things distributed are the patient. In both the above, the target is
the object of a spacetime postposition, but it can be marked by an abstract
genitive postposition as well:
or other abstract postpositions:
Contrast with:
ŝrun-twâ |
ĉul-i |
mâ-ĵĭn |
pen |
tu-i. |
music-say |
performance-at |
person-young |
all |
AGT-at |
All the children sang a song (the same song, in chorus). |
{mrel} comes at the beginning of an utterance and indicates that the speaker is
resuming a topic that has lain dormant for a while. I use it in my diary when
one diary entry continues the narrative of the same events from a previous one.
One might begin a conversation with {mrel} to remind the listener of a topic you
were talking about earlier. It could be translated as "As I was saying," or
"Remember what we were talking about earlier? I have more to say about it."
ĥy-i |
grĭ-fwa-zô |
mwe |
Ќ-ť |
tu-i. |
PAT-at |
function-CAUS-V.ACT |
IMP |
1-2 |
AGT-at |
As I was saying, we need to get the heating system fixed before winter. |
{men} is repeated after two or more phrases, or after the main verb or
postpositional phrase of two or more clauses, to express a series of contrasted
alternatives. It can be translated "on the one hand... on the other hand..."
though there is no arbitrary limit on how many {men}s one can have in a
sentence.
θuň |
krĭ-o |
lju-θaj-zô |
be |
men, |
krĭ-gjâ-zô |
men. |
story |
create-at |
read-OPP-V.ACT |
maybe |
ALT |
create-language-V.ACT |
ALT |
On one hand I might write a story, on the other hand I might do some conlanging. |
{ŝe} indicates that the preceding sentence constituent (noun phrase,
postpositional phrase, verb phrase) is in contrast to some other corresponding
constituent, either in the same clause or an earlier clause, or perhaps an implicit
contrast. Compare to the comparison postposition {dî'fu-i} and the contrastive
conjunction {ðe}.
ty |
ř |
ruŋ-zô |
ƴeŋ. |
home |
from |
go-V.ACT |
barely |
In those days I used to go lots of places, but nowadays, because of Covid, I barely leave home. |
ĥul |
jâ-o |
źǒ, |
cluŋ-cô-zô |
mwe |
ŝe. |
anger |
state-to |
IMP.NEG |
repaying.good.for.evil-OPP2-V.ACT |
IMP |
by.contrast |
Don't get mad, get even. |
{sem} is a postpositive clitic with a meaning similar to {ŝǒn, ʝon, wǒn}. It's not
used very often.
kujm |
pǒ |
o |
gî'sĭr-tôn |
ðĭ-i |
râ |
mĭ-i |
gju-Ł-zô |
źǒ |
sem. |
purpose |
DEM3 |
to |
spaghetti-GNR |
relationship-at |
event |
TOP-at |
speak-3.GEN-V.ACT |
IMP.NEG |
so |
So that's why we don't talk about the noodle incident. |
Evidentiality
The suffix {-pôm} derives evidentiality adverbs from root words or
compound stems referring to the source of information. Such
adverbs can be placed after a verb or a postpositional phrase,
or at the beginning of a sentence.
tam-ram-pôm | twâ-cu | pǒ | mĭ-i | ĵyn-fwa | heŋ | ŋĭn-i. |
Tom-NAME-EVD | sentence-system | DEM3 | TOP-at | interest-CAUS | not | CMT-at |
(Tom tells me that) that book isn't very interesting. |
Evidentiality marking is optional and actually fairly uncommon in gzb,
though I'm trying to make myself use it more often in my writing when
appropriate.
Use of {-pôm} to form validational adverbs
{-pôm} can also produce validational adverbs; words expressing the
speaker's degree or kind of certainty about what they are saying.
Sometimes the difference between a validational and evidential adverb
is blurry:
hyw | memory, experience |
hyw-pôm | I know this from experience / I remember this happening |
źy | dream |
źy-pôm | This happened in a dream / This didn't really happen, I just dreamed it, but it's interesting |
brâl | certainty |
brâl-pôm | I'm certain of this |
ĵrĭw | supposing, guessing |
ĵrĭw-pôm | I suppose, I guess that... |
(from the LCC2 translation relay text)
źy-ŋa-pôm | kyn-la-ĉu | ty-vir |
dream-ATD.surprise-EVD | parent-ATD.affection-two | home-far.in.front.of |
tyn-van | ₣âl-ĉa | kwǒ. | ~~~ |
place-V.STATE | sudden.change-tool | some | ... |
(I dreamed (astonishingly) that:) Way out in front of my parents' house was a transformation machine. |
(From a passage in my journal describing a dream weird even by dream standards.)
Use of {-pôm} to form attitudinal adverbs
When {-pôm} is used with a root
or stem for a mental state, it forms an adverb describing the
speaker's attitude to the situation described by the sentence or
utterance. No ambiguity with the use of {-pôm} to form
evidentiality adverbs is likely to result, as a mental state cannot be
construed as evidence for anything.
sjum-pôm | jĭlm-ĉa | ĥy-i | tru-zô. |
thankfulness-ATT | open-tool | PAT-at | find-V.ACT |
Thankfully, I found my keys. (= I found my keys; I feel thankfulness.) |
hěl'vĭ-pôm | ƥ | lĭw-i | lĭm | ĥy-i | ₣ĭ-dô-ƥ-zô |
contempt-ATT | 3 | relationship-at | friend | PAT-at | trust-violation-3-V.ACT |
Contemptibly, he betrayed his friend. (= He betrayed his friend; I feel contempt.) |
Interestingly, she has devised a conlang with fifty-three open
classes. (= She has devised a conlang with fifty-three open classes;
I want to know more.)
It appears, from a look at my online corpus, that I use {-pôm}
more often for attitudinals than for validationals, and more often for
validationals than for evidentials -- although {-pôm} was
originally used for evidentials, and its use was stretched to cover
validationals and attitudinals later. This is probably a function of
my primary uses of the language: in writing my journal, if gzb were
the sort of language where all indicative sentences are obligatorily
marked for evidentiality, the vast majority of sentences would be
marked as "direct experience" or "visual". But
occasions to obliquely mention my attitude to the situation expressed
by a sentence are far more frequent than occasions to mention how
(other than by direct experience, the pragmatic default in such a
context) I know about something. As for validationals, I find that
the most common (in)validational use of {-pôm} in my journal is
{źy-pôm}, "I dreamed this" (see example above). A
common pattern is to begin one paragraph with {źy-pôm} and
then, after recounting the dream, to begin another paragraph with
{hyw-pôm} "I experienced this".
Main {gjâ-zym-byn} index
Phonology
Derivational morphology
Semantics
History
Lexicon
Abbreviations used in the interlinear glosses
My conlang page
My home page
Last updated September 2023.